Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

New study reveals how language fuels U.S. political polarization

by Eric W. Dolan
February 5, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

In a recent series of studies, researchers have uncovered that the way political opinions are expressed—especially the use of generic language—can significantly amplify perceived differences between political parties in the United States. By examining how statements about party beliefs are presented and perceived, the studies provide evidence that broad, sweeping statements contribute to a heightened sense of polarization among the public. The findings have been published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS).

The motivation for this research sprang from a desire to understand the mechanisms driving the increasing political polarization observed in the U.S. over recent years. With political deadlock becoming more common, and social trust at a low, identifying the factors that contribute to these divisions is crucial. The researchers were particularly interested in exploring how language — specifically the use of generics in political discourse — might play a role in exacerbating these divides, a factor previously overlooked in studies of political polarization.

“I have been interested in research on partisan polarization, both ideological and affective (feelings towards the outparty, regardless of ideology) for several years,” said study author Gustavo Novoa, a PhD candidate in political science at Columbia University.

“Political scientists and political psychologists have produced a large literature on why people have become so polarized, but the fact that language had not been discussed as a reason was interesting to me and my colleagues. We thought it might play a role and we wanted to bring together two research areas that in the past had been distant from one another: the study of generic language and the study of mass partisan polarization.”

Novoa and his colleagues conducted a series of three studies to investigate the impact of generic language.

Study 1: Endorsement of Generic Claims

In the first study, the researchers aimed to investigate how endorsements of generic political statements could influence perceptions of polarization between the two major U.S. political parties. The study enlisted 417 participants, who were evenly split between self-identified Democrats and Republicans.

These participants were presented with various political stances and asked to estimate the level of support for these stances within each party. Following this, they were asked to express their agreement or disagreement with generic statements that represented those stances, such as “Democrats believe that we are spending too little on space exploration programs” or “Republicans believe that the federal income tax they pay is too high.”

The researchers found that participants were prone to overestimating the support for a policy within the party named in the generic statement and underestimating it within the opposite party. This tendency led to a perceived gap between the parties that was wider than what actual public opinion polls suggested.

Even small differences in how much Democrats and Republicans supported various policies led people to make broad generalizations. For example, if Democrats slightly more often supported a policy than Republicans, people would conclude “Democrats believe X” and “Republicans do not believe X,” even if the difference in support was small. This highlighted the potent role of generic language in exaggerating perceived differences between political parties, suggesting that such language could be contributing to the polarization observed in the political landscape.

“I was surprised by how small differences in the perceived preferences between Republicans and Democrats translated into large differences in the language that was endorsed to describe them,” Novoa told PsyPost.

Study 2: Memory for Generic Claims

The second study focused on how people recall political information, specifically examining whether there’s a tendency to remember quantified statements as generics. This time, 928 participants, divided again between Democrats and Republicans, were presented with both generic and quantified (specific) statements attributed to politicians. After being distracted by a brief task, they were asked to recall these statements.

The outcome of this study underscored a significant bias towards recalling information in generic terms, regardless of how it was initially presented. This was true for both generic and quantified statements, indicating that people’s memories default to recalling political information in broad, sweeping terms. This finding suggests that the simplification of political messages into generic terms may be a cognitive process contributing to the reinforcement and transmission of polarized views, as it promotes a less nuanced understanding of party positions.

“This suggests that even if politicians are careful to use more nuanced language, people will often remember their claims as generic, and therefore draw polarized inferences,” said study co-author Susan Gelman.

Study 3: Polarized Interpretation of Generic Claims

In the third study, the researchers explored whether exposure to generic versus quantified statements about political parties leads to polarized judgments. With 422 participants involved, the study presented fictional political statements in three forms: generic, “many,” and “some,” related to both Democrats and Republicans. (For example, “Democrats support House Bill B.937,” “Many Republicans are in favor of Title 9854,” and “Some Democrats oppose hosting the International Design Exhibition event.”)

Participants were then asked to estimate how prevalent they believed these attitudes or beliefs were within each party.

The results revealed that generics led to significantly exaggerated prevalence estimates for the named party in the statement and minimized estimates for the unnamed, opposing party, thereby widening the perceived ideological gap between them. This effect was notably stronger for generic statements than for those quantified with “many” or “some,” demonstrating that generic language not only fosters polarized interpretations but does so more effectively than language that includes quantification.

This phenomenon suggests that the manner in which political information is communicated can significantly influence the perception of polarization, driving a wedge between the understanding of party positions even further.

Implications of the Findings

Together, these findings shed light on the powerful role language plays in political discourse. By demonstrating that the use of generic language can amplify perceived differences between political parties, the research suggests a potential pathway through which political polarization is maintained and even deepened.

The findings provide evidence “that the use of generic language, common in everyday speech, has the potential to be interpreted to mean extreme prevalences where they might not exist,” Novoa told PsyPost. “For example, you might find examples in the media that argue that Democratic voters are in favor of raising taxes. This is true only in the sense that Democratic voters favor raising taxes at a greater rate than Republican voters. In reality, neither Republican nor Democrat voters support higher taxes. However, you make the statement that Democrats support higher taxes and Republicans do not, most people will agree when surveyed.”

Limitations and Future Research

While these studies offer valuable insights, they also come with limitations. Primarily, the research was conducted within the context of the U.S. political system, a two-party framework that may not directly apply to countries with multiparty systems or different levels of polarization.

“Because we only studied the U.S. political context, we don’t know how similarly these findings apply in the political environments of other countries,” Novoa explained. “We also don’t know how generic language may affect the perception of intra-party subgroups.”

Furthermore, the focus on generics within political discourse leaves open questions about how these findings might translate to other domains or political environments. Future research could explore the operation of generics in non-political contexts, examine the effects in multiparty systems, and investigate whether these patterns of language use and perception are unique to polarized political landscapes or are more universally applicable.

“We are continuing this research and further exploring questions in this area,” Novoa said.

The study, “Generically partisan: Polarization in political communication“, was authored by Gustavo Novoa, Margaret Echelbarger, Andrew Gelman, and Susan A. Gelman.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

People with psychopathic traits fail to learn from painful outcomes
Narcissism

National narcissism linked to emotional impairments and dehumanization, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A new study suggests that people who see their nation as uniquely important often struggle with recognizing emotions and experience more anger and contempt—factors that may help explain why they’re more likely to dehumanize both outsiders and fellow citizens.

Read moreDetails
Fascinating study reveals how Trump’s moral rhetoric diverges from common Republican language
Donald Trump

Viral AI-images highlight how Trump engages in “victimcould,” scholar argues

July 6, 2025

How can one of the world's most powerful men also be its biggest victim? A new paper argues it’s a political strategy based on hypothetical, not actual, harm—a concept the author calls “victimcould” used to justify present-day aggression.

Read moreDetails
New study suggests Donald Trump’s “fake news” attacks are backfiring
Political Psychology

Scientists are uncovering more and more unsettling facts about our politics

July 5, 2025

Why has politics become so personal? The answers may lie in our minds. These 13 studies from the new science of political behavior reveal the hidden psychological forces—from personality to primal fear—that are driving us further apart.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Political Psychology

Despite political tensions, belief in an impending U.S. civil war remains low

July 4, 2025

A new national survey finds that only a small fraction of Americans believe civil war is likely or necessary.

Read moreDetails
Racial and religious differences help explain why unmarried voters lean Democrat
Political Psychology

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

July 3, 2025

Americans with student loan debt are more likely to vote and engage in political activities than those without debt, likely because they see government as responsible and capable of addressing their financial burden through policy change.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Mental Health

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

July 3, 2025

Do conservatives really have better mental well-being than liberals? A new study suggests the answer depends entirely on how you ask. The well-known ideological gap disappears when "mental health" is replaced with the less-stigmatized phrase "overall mood."

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Being adopted doesn’t change how teens handle love and dating

Probiotics show promise for reducing hyperactivity in young children with autism and ADHD

Number of children affected by parental substance use has surged to 19 million, study finds

National narcissism linked to emotional impairments and dehumanization, new study finds

Personality may be a key factor connecting negative parenting experiences to adult challenges

New research reveals emotional control deficits in generalized anxiety disorder

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

Positive attitudes toward AI linked to more prone to problematic social media use

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy