Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

New study reveals unexpected downsides to expressing gratitude in close relationships

by Mane Kara-Yakoubian
May 6, 2024
in Social Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

In a curious twist to the traditional narrative surrounding gratitude, recent research published in the Journal of Personality & Social Psychology proposes that expressing thanks might not always be beneficial.

Gratitude, often encapsulated in a simple “thank you,” is universally recognized as a positive response that strengthens social bonds and interpersonal relationships. Extensive research has documented its benefits, from enhancing emotional connections to fostering mutual cooperation within various social settings. However, the act of expressing gratitude, particularly within close relationships, can sometimes yield unexpectedly negative reactions, complicating our understanding of its social utility.

In Study 1, researchers Jiaqi Yu and Shereen J. Chaudhry analyzed 335 real-world acceptance speeches from major film and music ceremonies in China and the United States, focusing on whether awardees thanked their close others during these public acknowledgments. The study aimed to capture naturally occurring expressions of gratitude in a high-profile setting. The analysis coded speeches for mentions of gratitude towards close relatives and friends, comparing the frequency between the two cultural contexts. Results showed a significant cultural difference: 19.3% of Chinese awardees thanked close others, versus 71.1% of American awardees.

Study 2 involved 273 participants who were presented with a choice between recording a personal audio thank-you message for a close other or completing a neutral filler task. This design tested the inclination to express gratitude directly in a controlled, yet personal context. The key measure was the choice to send a thank-you message versus choosing the filler task. The findings revealed that only 32% of participants from China chose to send a thank-you message, compared to 56.7% of participants from the United States, indicating a cultural reservation towards direct expressions of gratitude.

Study 3 presented 402 participants with various hypothetical scenarios where an individual performed a favor for them. The scenarios varied by the relational closeness of the favor doer (close other vs. distant other). Participants assessed their likelihood of expressing gratitude in each scenario, ranging from -50 (extremely unlikely to express gratitude) to +50 (extremely likely to express gratitude). The results confirmed that Chinese participants were less likely to express gratitude towards close others than Americans, highlighting the impact of relational expectations on gratitude expressions.

Study 4 recruited 94 participants, examining both the emotional response of gratitude and its outward expression following hypothetical favors. Participants rated their feelings of gratitude and likelihood of expressing gratitude across situations involving both close and distant others. Similar to previous findings, Chinese participants reported lower levels of both feeling and expressing gratitude towards close others, contrasting with the American participants’ responses.

In Study 5, 199 participants evaluated how much they viewed various actions by close others as duties, using a scale from -30 (definitely not a duty) to +30 (definitely a duty). This study aimed to understand cultural differences in perceived obligations within close relationships. Participants assessed a range of scenarios where a close other performed different actions, gauging whether these actions were seen as obligations or favors. The findings revealed that Chinese participants generally perceived a higher level of duty in these actions than Americans, indicating stronger expectations of close others in Chinese culture.

Study 6 involved 314 participants who were presented with scenarios where the cultural perception of duty for a given favor varied. Participants once again rated their likelihood of expressing gratitude. This study tested the hypothesis that cultural differences in the perception of relational duties would affect gratitude expression. The results showed that when duties were culturally agreed upon, gratitude expression differed minimally between cultures. However, when there was cultural disagreement about duties, Chinese participants were significantly less likely to express gratitude than Americans, highlighting the impact of cultural expectations on gratitude behaviors.

In the final experiment, 189 participants rated their preference for direct versus indirect gratitude expressions in interactions with close and distant others, using a scale from -30 (strong preference for indirect expressions) to +30 (strong preference for direct expressions). Chinese participants preferred less direct expressions of gratitude towards close others compared to Americans, suggesting that direct expressions might imply a lesser degree of intimacy in Chinese culture. This preference was not observed in interactions involving distant others, where the cultural differences were minimal.

Across these experiments, the consistent pattern suggests that cultural and relational expectations profoundly influence how gratitude is expressed and perceived. In more collectivist cultures like China, expressing gratitude to close others may imply a lack of inherent relational closeness, which contrasts with individualistic cultures like the United States where gratitude is more frequently and openly expressed.

One limitation is the focus on China and the United States, which may not represent the full spectrum of individualistic and collectivist societies. Thus, these findings might not extend to cultures that have different relational norms and expectations that influence the expression of gratitude.

The research, “‘Thanks, but No Thanks’: Gratitude Expression Paradoxically Signals Distance”, was authored by Jiaqi Yu and Shereen J. Chaudhry.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin2ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Dark personality traits linked to “social zapping”: New study examines people who cancel plans at the last minute
Narcissism

Narcissistic individuals are more prone to maladaptive daydreaming

July 14, 2025

A new study suggests that narcissistic personality traits are linked to maladaptive daydreaming, with psychological defense mechanisms playing a key role in the relationship. The findings highlight how different defense styles may influence a person’s tendency to escape into fantasy.

Read moreDetails
Daughters who feel more attractive report stronger, more protective bonds with their fathers
Artificial Intelligence

People who use AI may pay a social price, according to new psychology research

July 14, 2025

Worried that using AI tools like ChatGPT at work makes you look lazy? New research suggests you might be right. A study finds employees who use AI are often judged more harshly, facing negative perceptions about their competence and effort.

Read moreDetails
New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election
Anxiety

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election

July 13, 2025

Among young adults, stress from election news was linked to higher risks of depression and anxiety, while pre-election anticipatory stress was linked to depression only. Stress about the election outcome was not associated with either condition.

Read moreDetails
Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Psychopathy

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

July 12, 2025

Psychopathic traits alone don’t always lead to serious offending—but when combined with weak self-regulation, they may significantly raise the risk. A new study reveals how these factors interact to shape property crime patterns in adolescents already involved in the justice system.

Read moreDetails
Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Political Psychology

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

July 12, 2025

New research finds that students with different political identities tend to engage in slightly different everyday behaviors. But students dramatically overestimate how much liberals and conservatives differ, fueling a distorted sense of social and political division.

Read moreDetails
A surprising body part might provide key insights into schizophrenia risk
Machiavellianism

Massive psychology study reveals disturbing truths about Machiavellian leaders

July 11, 2025

For employees working under a manipulative boss, the damage is clear: lower job satisfaction and higher burnout. A study in the Journal of Organizational Behavior confirms this toxic impact, yet reveals these leaders aren't always penalized for their destructive behavior.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns
Psychopathy

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

July 10, 2025

What if the roots of psychopathy could be traced in our blood? New research is looking beyond psychology and into our endocrine system. A new study suggests hormones like cortisol and testosterone may hold important clues to a person's manipulative and impulsive tendencies.

Read moreDetails
Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds
Political Psychology

Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds

July 10, 2025

What if the key to swaying a swing voter lies in their biology? New research found giving testosterone to weakly affiliated Democratic men made them less loyal to their party and more open to Republicans, revealing a potential hormonal link to political persuasion.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

The triglyceride-glucose index: Can it predict depression risk in the elderly?

People with ADHD exhibit altered brain activity before making high-stakes choices

Narcissistic individuals are more prone to maladaptive daydreaming

Effort or fate? Sexual mindsets predict whether bedroom bliss spills over into broader wellbeing

People who use AI may pay a social price, according to new psychology research

Researchers identify a shared brain pattern behind feeling surprised

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election

Methylphenidate: ADHD drug curbs impulsivity in men only, linked to brain wiring differences

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy