Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

No gender bias in voter reactions to political flip-flopping, study finds

by Eric W. Dolan
May 22, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A recent study published in the Journal of Experimental Political Science investigates the impact of political repositioning on candidate evaluations, focusing on whether gender influences these perceptions. Conducted in Flanders, the research finds that politicians who frequently change their policy stances are viewed less favorably by the public, regardless of gender.

The motivation behind this study stems from the dual expectations placed on political parties. On one hand, parties are expected to maintain clear, stable policy positions, which fosters trust and ideological clarity. On the other hand, parties need to be responsive and adaptable to changing public opinions and circumstances.

Previous research has shown that changing policy stances, known as repositioning, can damage a politician’s reputation. However, it remained unclear if this reputational cost is gendered. Given that female politicians are often perceived as more honest and reliable, the study hypothesized that women might face harsher judgment for changing their positions, potentially violating these positive stereotypes.

“I became interested in the consequences of party repositioning for party reputations because there seems to be tension between two core elements of political representation,” said study author Maurits J. Meijers, an assistant professor of political science at Radboud University in Nijmegen.

“On the one hand, we want parties to have clearly identifiable policy profiles. We want to know what parties stand for and we want parties to be committed to their ideological beliefs. This would require parties to be steadfast in their policy positioning.”

“At the same time, we want parties to be responsive to public opinion. Wlezien and Soroka’s thermostatic model of representation posits that parties should adapt their positioning in line with public opinion. Moreover, we want parties to be flexible enough to adjust their positions when political or economic circumstances change. This would require parties to have considerable leeway in their policy positioning.”

“My project ‘Credible’ or ‘Capricious’? The Reputational Cost of Party Policy Change funded by the Dutch Research Council addresses how citizens evaluate this trade-off between stable and credible positioning vs. flexibility.”

“The present study addresses this question from the perspective of gendered candidate evaluations. We know from research on gendered evaluations of politicians that female politicians are considered to be more sincere and honest. Political psychology work on repositioning has shown that when politicians change their positions people believe these politicians to be less sincere and honest.”

The participants were recruited from a Flemish panel of users of the “Election Compass,” a voting advice application. The final sample included 6,957 respondents, although it was initially intended to be 4,000, with quotas for age, gender, and education to ensure representativeness.

The experiment had a 2×2 factorial design, manipulating two variables: the gender of the candidate (male or female) and the frequency of their policy position changes (frequent or infrequent). Participants were shown a simulated news report about a candidate’s performance, including a photo and first name to indicate gender, and information about their tendency to change positions on key issues such as childcare, climate policy, and immigration policy.

The repositioning scenario indicated that the candidate frequently changed positions on three issues: childcare, climate policy, and immigration policy. These issues were selected to cover a range of stereotypically feminine and masculine domains. Participants rated the candidates on overall evaluation, perceived trustworthiness, honesty, decisiveness, competence, and voting intention.

The study revealed that candidates who frequently changed their policy positions were evaluated less positively than those who did so infrequently. This negative perception extended to trustworthiness and voting intentions. Candidates who frequently repositioned were viewed as less honest, decisive, and competent.

“My study – a vignette survey experiment conducted in Flanders, Belgium – found that politicians who frequently change their positions are trusted less and are punished electorally,” Meijers told PsyPost. “Flip flopping politicians are also seen as less honest and competent. The largest effect was for decisiveness: flipflopping politicians are perceived to be less decisive.”

Contrary to the initial hypothesis, the data showed no significant difference in the negative impact of repositioning based on the candidate’s gender. Both male and female candidates who frequently changed positions were evaluated similarly, suggesting that gender stereotypes did not significantly influence these evaluations in the context of Flanders.

“I was surprised to see the lack of a gendered effect,” Meijers said. “Especially in light of previous evidence of the effects of stereotype violations provided by Erin Cassese and Mirya Holman.”

“It is possible, however, that the negative effects of stereotype violations for female politicians were cancelled out by stereotype violations for male politicians. Male politicians are stereotypically seen as more decisive, and repositioning politicians were punished most for decisiveness. That said, I did not see gender differences in the evaluations of any of the traits.”

Meijers conducted several robustness checks, including post-stratification weights to address sample representativeness and control for attentiveness. These checks confirmed the initial findings, reinforcing that frequent repositioning carries a reputational cost, but this cost does not vary significantly between male and female candidates.

But the study includes some caveats to consider. For instance, it was conducted in Flanders, a region with stringent gender quotas and relatively low levels of gender stereotyping in politics. These contextual factors may limit the generalizability of the findings to other regions with different political dynamics and levels of gender stereotyping.

Future research could address these limitations by replicating the study in different political contexts, including regions with higher levels of gender stereotyping and different electoral systems. Further studies could also explore the impact of repositioning on specific policy issues perceived as stereotypically masculine or feminine, as well as investigate how explicit activation of gender stereotypes during campaigns influences voter evaluations.

“In the medium-term, there are still a couple of studies from the Dutch Science Council project in the pipeline,” Meijers said. “With Ruth Dassonneville (University of Montreal), I have conducted a study in which individual characteristics explain whether citizens believe repositioning to be legitimate in a representative democracy. With Mariken van der Velden (Free University Amsterdam), I have studied how voters respond to compromises made during coalition negotiations. Lastly, I have conducted a survey experiment in three countries to see how issue ownership and affective polarization affect citizens’ evaluations of position changes.”

“In the longer term, I want to continue this line of research, which studies how the political behaviour of political elites affect citizens’ trust in and commitment to democracy. For instance, I am currently working on a project to see how citizens perceive lying politicians and how this affects their views on democracy.”

The study, “The Way She Moves: Political Repositioning and Gender Stereotypes,” was published online on April 25, 2024.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes
Narcissism

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

June 13, 2025

New research published in Behavioral Sciences shows that certain narcissistic traits are linked to anti-immigrant attitudes through competitive worldviews and ideological beliefs, highlighting the role of personality in shaping how people view immigrants and social hierarchies.

Read moreDetails
Uncertainty about immigration intensifies nationalist sentiments
Political Psychology

From well-being to white replacement: What psychology research says about immigration

June 11, 2025

Immigration sparks fierce debates—but what does the research say? These seven studies uncover surprising findings about mental health, crime, social media, nationalism, and political violence.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists identify key gatekeeper of human consciousness

New study links intermittent fasting to improved mood via brain’s dopamine system

Ashwagandha extract boosts memory and cognition in people with mild cognitive impairment, study finds

Similarity in long‑term romantic couples probably matters less than we think

TikTok and similar platforms linked to body dissatisfaction and eating disorder symptoms

Out-of-body experiences linked to higher rates of mental health symptoms and trauma, study finds

Attachment anxiety might explain how early trauma shapes emotions during sexual disagreements

Support for war is associated with narcissistic personality traits

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy