Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Business

People are less tolerant of atheists expressing their beliefs at work compared to Christians, Muslims, or Jews

by Beth Ellwood
August 2, 2021
in Business, Psychology of Religion
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook

According to a study published in the journal Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, people discriminate against atheists when it comes to the expression of beliefs in the workplace. Across three studies, people were consistently less likely to say they would allow an atheist — compared to a Christian, Muslim, or Jew — to sport a symbol of their beliefs in the workplace. The researchers further found that this effect was motivated by the assumption that the atheist employees were trying to impose their beliefs on others.

The study’s authors Kimberly Rios and her team describe atheists as “one of the most disliked groups in the United States.” This contempt for atheists includes stereotypes that atheists are immoral and untrustworthy which leads them to be discriminated against in contexts like politics and on the job market. There is some evidence to suggest that atheists are even more stigmatized in the U.S. than religious minorities like Muslims and Jews.

“We were interested in whether and why atheists’ requests would be accommodated less frequently in the workplace than those of other (religious) groups for a few reasons. First, although a small number of studies have suggested that atheists are less likely than religious individuals to be hired for certain types of jobs, no research to date had examined how atheists (relative to religious individuals) are perceived in the workplace – that is, after they are hired,” explained Rios, an associate professor of psychology at Ohio University.

“Most notably, however, we wanted to compare people’s responses to atheists and to particular religious groups. Prior work has looked at perceptions of atheists versus religious individuals in general, without specifying the religion. In the United States, where Christianity is both the dominant and majority religion, people may tend to think ‘Christian’ when hearing the term ‘religious.’ But are they less likely to accommodate atheist employees’ workplace requests than not only those of Christian and Jewish employees, but also those of Muslim employees?”

“That is a particularly interesting question because both atheists and Muslims are among the most negatively stereotyped groups in American society,” Rios said. “For example, public opinion polls show that many Americans are unlikely to vote for an atheist or Muslim political candidate, nor do they want their children to marry atheists or Muslims. In everyday workplace situations, though, is one of these groups met with more backlash, and if so, why?”

Rios and her colleagues conducted three studies where respondents read vignettes describing either an atheist, Christian, Jewish, or Muslim employee who wanted to display a symbol of their beliefs in the workplace (e.g., a belief quote inside their cubicle). After reading these scenarios, the participants were asked to rate how willing they would be to accommodate the employee’s request to express their beliefs at work.

“We looked at belief-related requests (instead of, say, willingness to give employees a raise or promotion) because we thought such requests would be less susceptible to social desirability concerns,” Rios said. “That is, an employer could explain away their reluctance to accommodate the expression of certain beliefs (or lack of beliefs) on the grounds that such beliefs should simply be kept out of the workplace… whereas it’s difficult to justify promoting or giving a raise to a member of one group over another, without seeming biased against the latter group.”

In the first study, the researchers found that respondents were less likely to accommodate an atheist (compared to a Christian, Jew, or Muslim) in expressing their beliefs inside their office cubicle or on their clothing (i.e., by wearing a pin).

A second study replicated the above effect and further found evidence for why people tend to discriminate in this way. Participants were more likely to feel that the atheist (compared to the religious employees) was displaying their beliefs in order to push their views on others — suggesting that the atheists posed a greater symbolic threat compared to the Christians, Jews, or Muslims. Moreover, symbolic threat mediated the relationship between the target’s religiosity and participants’ willingness to accommodate them in displaying a belief quote or wearing a symbolic pin.

In a third study, the participants were again less accepting of an employee wearing a belief pin when he/she was an atheist compared to a religious person. And again, this effect was partly explained by the belief that the atheist employee was pushing his/her views onto others. In addition, the effect was also explained (but to a lesser extent) by the belief that the atheist was posing a threat to others’ well-being or access to resources — what the researchers termed a “realistic threat.” An example of a realistic threat was believing an employee wearing a belief pin would cause a restaurant to lose its customers.

“Participants perceived the atheist employees, more so than the Christian, Muslim, and Jewish employees, as wanting to impose their beliefs onto the workplace – in other words, as a ‘symbolic threat’ to workplace values. To a lesser extent, participants also saw the atheist employees as a ‘realistic threat’ to the workplace (i.e., as jeopardizing the company’s economic status and/or the employees’ general well-being). Both these types of threat helped explain people’s reticence to accommodate the atheist employees’ requests,” Rios told PsyPost.

Across all three studies, atheists were given a harder time for expressing their beliefs than Christians, Jews, or Muslims were. According to the study authors, this is in line with the perception that atheists represent an outgroup, while religious groups with similar Abrahamic roots are seen as belonging to the ingroup of religious people. When analyzing the three studies together, the researchers found that religious people were the least willing to accommodate atheists’ requests to express their beliefs. It is likely that for religious respondents, the Christians, Jews, and Muslims were seen as part of the ingroup, while atheists were not.

“This likely has to do with the fact that Christian, Jewish, and Muslim individuals all fall under the broader umbrella of ‘religious,’ whereas atheists are a separate category altogether. Indeed, a combined analysis across all our studies showed that participants who self-identified as religious were especially unwilling to accommodate atheists relative to other groups,” Rios explained.

Rios and her team say that a helpful approach to reducing these anti-atheist attitudes might be to emphasize the value of respecting others’ beliefs in the workplace — religious or otherwise. They also suggest that explaining that diversity is beneficial to the success of a workplace might help eliminate the perception that atheists are a threat to the well-being of the company. The authors note that their study was limited since the participants were not real supervisors and were only reading vignettes. They propose that future studies should examine these effects among real employers to strengthen their findings.

“Now that American society is becoming more secular (with church attendance recently falling below 50% for the first time), it will be critical to determine whether and to what extent our results hold up over time,” Rios said.

The study, “Explaining Anti-Atheist Discrimination in the Workplace: The Role of Intergroup Threat”, was authored by Kimberly Rios, Leah R. Halper, and Christopher P. Scheitle.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Neuroforecasting: New research shows brain activity can predict crowd preferences
Business

Neuroforecasting: New research shows brain activity can predict crowd preferences

May 21, 2025

A new study reveals that brain activity, particularly in regions linked to emotion, predicts market preferences more accurately than self-reported choices—especially when samples aren’t demographically representative. Neural signals offered consistent forecasts even when behavioral data failed.

Read moreDetails
Psychedelics linked to religious disaffiliation—but not spiritual change—in large-scale study
Psychedelic Drugs

Psychedelics linked to religious disaffiliation—but not spiritual change—in large-scale study

May 13, 2025

Psychedelics are often described as spiritual tools—but do they change people’s religious beliefs? A large new study found that while psychedelic use was associated with religious disaffiliation, it did not cause lasting changes in religiosity or spirituality over time.

Read moreDetails
A familiar prejudice may have more to do with “identity signaling”
Psychology of Religion

A familiar prejudice may have more to do with “identity signaling”

May 1, 2025

A new study finds that negative views of atheists may be less about moral disagreement and more about signaling group identity.

Read moreDetails
Unpredictable childhoods may shape how people relate to God
Attachment Styles

Unpredictable childhoods may shape how people relate to God

April 30, 2025

A new study suggests that growing up in unpredictable environments can shape how people relate to God. The findings show that early instability is linked to insecure attachment to God, which may reduce feelings of divine forgiveness and limit the use of religious coping strategies.

Read moreDetails
People intuitively associate religiosity with goodness and atheism with wrongdoing
Psychology of Religion

People intuitively associate religiosity with goodness and atheism with wrongdoing

April 27, 2025

A new study suggests that moral biases favoring religious individuals over atheists are widespread, even in predominantly secular societies.

Read moreDetails
New study sheds light on what helps nonbelievers thrive psychologically in a religious culture
Psychology of Religion

New study sheds light on what helps nonbelievers thrive psychologically in a religious culture

April 25, 2025

A new study explores how nonbelievers in Poland maintain psychological well-being in a largely religious culture, highlighting the roles of social support, emotional regulation, and self-clarity.

Read moreDetails
Political doxing in the hiring process: New study reveals impact on job candidate evaluations
Autism

Why people with autism struggle to get hired

April 23, 2025

New research shows that social behaviors often misunderstood by interviewers can overshadow qualifications, leading to unfair hiring decisions.

Read moreDetails
Agnostics are more indecisive, neurotic, and prone to maximizing choices, distinguishing them from atheists and Christians
Psychology of Religion

Agnostics are more indecisive, neurotic, and prone to maximizing choices, distinguishing them from atheists and Christians

April 11, 2025

Far from a midpoint between belief and disbelief, agnosticism shows a unique psychological makeup, new research published in Self & Identity suggests.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists challenge “dopamine detox” trend with evidence from avoidance learning

Non-right-handedness is more common across multiple mental health conditions

Withdrawal symptoms are common after stopping antidepressants

Anxious attachment linked to depression spillover in romantic relationships, study finds

Scientists find 16 new Alzheimer’s-linked genes using multi-ancestry genome data

Groundbreaking AI model uncovers hidden patterns of political bias in online news

Gut bacteria can influence how brain proteins are modified by carbohydrates

“Tiger mom” parenting boosts teens’ cognitive skills but undermines emotional development, study suggests

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy