Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

“Political contamination” can affect everyday choices in surprising ways

by Eric W. Dolan
December 1, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

t turns out that your political views might influence more than your vote—they could even change how you feel about chocolate. A new study published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin reveals that people rate products less favorably when they are associated with their least-liked political party. The findings highlight how “political contamination” subtly shapes our everyday preferences in surprising ways.

Political polarization, especially “affective polarization,” has been increasing across the globe. This form of polarization refers to the tendency to feel positive toward one’s political in-group while harboring distrust or dislike toward the out-group.

While this dynamic has been well-studied in contexts like voting and media consumption, its effect on neutral, everyday decisions has received less attention. The researchers behind this new study set out to explore how even apolitical products can become contaminated by political associations.

“I have always been interested in moral social cognition, and my interest in political psychology probably began many years ago when I observed a post on social media where two famous Swedish politicians from ideologically distant parties posted a selfie at a soccer game,” said study author Arvid Erlandsson, a senior associate professor at Linköping University and member of the JEDI Lab.

“The caption read something like, ‘There are many issues where we disagree, but today we are happy to be on the same side.’ About half the comments were positive, but the other half were negative, with some people saying they could never cheer for a team supported by their least-liked politician. This inspired me to systematically investigate whether non-political things can be ‘contaminated’ simply by being liked by one’s political out-group.”

To investigate this, the researchers conducted a series of four experiments involving a total of 3,985 participants.

In the first experiment, participants evaluated clothing items before and after learning that these items were worn by politicians from either their most-liked or least-liked political parties. The researchers used photographs of well-known Swedish politicians dressed in formal attire, alongside non-political individuals in similar outfits as a control.

The findings revealed a clear “negative out-group effect”: clothes worn by politicians from participants’ least-liked parties were rated as less attractive after the political association was revealed. A smaller but significant “positive in-group effect” was also observed, with clothing linked to the participants’ most-liked politicians receiving higher ratings.

The second experiment expanded the investigation to chocolates, a less identity-relevant product. Participants rated a selection of chocolate brands before and after being told that certain chocolates were favored by supporters of their most-liked or least-liked political parties. This experiment also included a condition where chocolates were linked to bipartisan preferences, as well as a control group with no political association.

The results replicated the negative out-group effect: chocolates linked to the least-liked party were rated less favorably after the association was revealed. However, the positive in-group effect was less consistent, suggesting that political associations have a stronger impact when linked to disliked groups than to favored ones.

The third experiment examined real financial decisions. Participants were asked to allocate monetary donations to pairs of well-known Swedish charities. In the experimental conditions, participants were informed that certain charities were more popular among supporters of their most-liked or least-liked political parties.

The researchers found that participants consistently donated less to charities associated with their least-liked party, confirming the negative out-group effect. However, there was no significant increase in donations to charities linked to participants’ most-liked parties, reinforcing the asymmetry observed in the earlier studies.

“Data collection for Studies 2 and 3 coincided with the Russian invasion of Ukraine, which united all Swedish political parties in their condemnation of Russia,” Erlandsson told PsyPost. “The fact that we still observed these effects during this period of cross-partisan friendliness made me even more convinced of the robustness of the findings.”

The final experiment explored the role of public versus private decision-making. Participants were asked to choose between products—including chocolates, fruits, and charities—in scenarios where their choices were either private or observed by in-group members (represented by virtual avatars).

The findings showed that the negative out-group effect was amplified in public settings. Participants were even less likely to choose products associated with their least-liked party when they believed their choices were being observed by others from their political in-group. Interestingly, public observation did not enhance the positive in-group effect, which remained weak or absent in this study.

“Earlier studies on affective political polarization have shown that people distance themselves from the political out-group, such as when dating, evaluating candidates for a job, or attending Thanksgiving dinners,” Erlandsson explained. “Other studies have demonstrated the ‘party-over-policy effect,’ where the same policy is evaluated positively if proposed by the political in-group and negatively if proposed by the out-group. We also know that people sometimes boycott explicitly partisan brands or products, such as Twitter/X under Elon Musk, who openly endorses Donald Trump.”

“Our study extends these findings by showing a new manifestation of political polarization: distancing from neutral and apolitical products. Additionally, we find that this tendency is stronger in public than in private situations, suggesting that people may avoid products linked to their political out-group for reputational reasons.”

Interestingly, while the negative out-group effect was robust across all studies, the positive in-group effect was weaker and less consistent, appearing primarily in the context of aesthetic evaluations (experiment 1) and certain chocolate ratings (experiment 2).

“This was somewhat surprising but aligns with the idea that, unlike other social identities, a negative political identity (opposition to party X) is more prominent than a positive political identity (support for party Y),” Erlandsson said.

“In all studies, we tested whether the effects were politically symmetrical—similar for left-leaning and right-leaning participants—or asymmetrical. In three of the four studies, the results were symmetrical, suggesting that the tendency to distance oneself from politically contaminated products exists across partisan lines. In one study, however, rightist participants, but not leftist participants, showed a positive in-group effect, liking clothes worn by in-group politicians.”

While the findings strongly suggest that political polarization influences everyday choices, the study has some limitations. Conducted in Sweden, a country with a multi-party system, the results may differ in two-party systems like the United States. Additionally, the study focused on relatively neutral products; future research could explore whether similar effects occur with identity-relevant choices, such as favorite sports teams or cultural symbols.

“The research reported here was funded by the Swedish Research Council, which also supports several other related projects we are working on,” Erlandsson added. “We are currently studying political discrimination, politically motivated reasoning, openness to opposing views, and reactions to prosocial and antisocial behaviors toward political out-groups.”

The study, “Politically Contaminated Clothes, Chocolates, and Charities: Distancing From Neutral Products Liked by Out-Group or In-Group Partisans,” was authored by Arvid Erlandsson, Artur Nilsson, Jennifer Rosander, Rebecka Persson, and Leaf Van Boven.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes
Narcissism

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

June 13, 2025

New research published in Behavioral Sciences shows that certain narcissistic traits are linked to anti-immigrant attitudes through competitive worldviews and ideological beliefs, highlighting the role of personality in shaping how people view immigrants and social hierarchies.

Read moreDetails
Uncertainty about immigration intensifies nationalist sentiments
Political Psychology

From well-being to white replacement: What psychology research says about immigration

June 11, 2025

Immigration sparks fierce debates—but what does the research say? These seven studies uncover surprising findings about mental health, crime, social media, nationalism, and political violence.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

A common parasite not only invades the brain — it can also decapitate human sperm

Almost all unmarried pregant women say that the fetus resembles the father, study finds

New neuroscience research reveals brain antioxidant deficit in depression

Scientists uncover kidney-to-brain route for Parkinson’s-related protein spread

Scientists reveal a surprising link between depression and microbes in your mouth

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

Experienced FPS gamers show faster, more efficient eye movements during aiming tasks, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy