Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Repeating the “F” word can improve threshold for pain during an ice water challenge

by Beth Ellwood
May 21, 2020
in Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: Craig Sunter)

(Photo credit: Craig Sunter)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A recent study found that repeating the “F” word during an ice water experiment increased subjects’ tolerance and threshold for pain. However, reciting made-up swear words showed no such pain-reducing effects. The study was published in Frontiers in Psychology.

Numerous studies have shown that the use of swear words can strengthen pain tolerance during an ice water experiment. UK researchers Stephens and Robertson set out to explore the mechanism behind this pain-relieving effect in a unique way. A team of specialists invented two new “swear” words with properties similar to known curse words. They then tested the invented words in a cold pressor experiment to see whether they would mimic the pain-reducing effects of known swear words.

One theory suggests that swearing produces analgesic effects through autonomic arousal caused by increased emotion. To explore this idea, researchers chose the made-up swear word “fouch”, selected for its emotion-provoking potential. Another theory suggests that swearing alleviates pain by distracting attention away from the painful event. Researchers accordingly chose the second made-up curse word “twizpipe”, for its potential to evoke distraction through humor.

A total of 92 individuals with a mean age of 27 took part in an experiment that included four conditions. Subjects were asked to submerge one hand in an ice water bath maintained at 3-5° C and were instructed to repeat a designated word while keeping their hand in the water. Depending on the condition, subjects were allocated either a neutral word (a word used to describe a table), the “F” word, the invented swear word “fouch”, or the invented swear word “twizpipe.” Subjects were asked to report when they begin to feel pain (an indication of pain threshold) and to only remove their hand once the pain became unbearable (an indication of pain tolerance).

Results replicated previous findings showing that repeating the “F” word increased participants’ tolerance for pain. In addition, results demonstrated for the first time that repeating the “F” word also increased subjects’ threshold for pain.

Surprisingly, reciting the made-up swear words of “fouch” and “twizpipe” did not increase pain tolerance nor pain threshold. The authors explain that the lack of pain-alleviating effects from the invented swear words, while unexpected, is not wholly surprising. “While it is not properly understood how swear words gain their power,” the authors say, “it has been suggested that swearing is learned during childhood and that aversive classical conditioning contributes to the emotionally arousing aspects of swear word use.”

While the words “fouch” and “twizpipe” were selected for their ability to evoke emotion and humour similar to traditional swear words, “these properties did not facilitate pain alleviation effects, suggesting that surface properties of swear words (such as how they sound) do not explain the hypoalgesic effects of swearing.”

Researchers also tested whether certain properties of swear words could explain the beneficial effect of swearing on pain tolerance. Following each trial, participants rated the word they had been asked to repeat with respect to the concepts of humor, emotion, and distraction. After mediation analysis, it was found that none of these three properties mediated the effects of swearing on pain tolerance.

Researchers point out the significance of these null findings. “While swearing was rated as distracting (more so than the other words) the level of distraction was not related to the pain alleviation effect.” The authors continue, “distraction may not be important in explaining how swearing produces hypoalgesic effect.”

The researchers conclude that their study provides new evidence that swearing can boost the threshold for pain, but that mediation analysis did not provide insight into the properties of swear words that make analgesic effects possible. The authors address the limitation that their study made no distinction between the type of emotion elicited by the words, and suggest that future research assess both positive and negative emotional arousal when it comes to swearing.

The study, “Swearing as a Response to Pain: Assessing Hypoalgesic Effects of Novel “Swear” Words”, was authored by Richard Stevens and Olly Robertson.

RELATED

The way you blink reveals how music is shaping your attention, new study finds
Cognitive Science

The way you blink reveals how music is shaping your attention, new study finds

September 16, 2025
“A concerning finding”: New study on fake news detection uncovers a disturbing trend
Cognitive Science

New study links cognitive style to health misinformation detection

September 15, 2025
Psychology researchers identify a “burnout to extremism” pipeline
Cognitive Science

Cognitive ability becomes increasingly stable after age 3, study finds

September 15, 2025
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Cognitive Science

The origin of the mental number line may be biological, not cultural, according to a new study

September 11, 2025
Childhood adversity may blunt brain development rather than speed it up
Cognitive Science

Biological aging predicts midlife cognitive decline, especially in those raised in poverty

September 8, 2025
Extraverts show faster, stronger, and more patterned emotional reactions
Cognitive Science

Overconfidence in bullshit detection linked to cognitive blind spots and narcissistic traits

September 5, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Cognitive Science

Simple rhythmic sounds can reshape the brain’s entire network landscape, study finds

September 2, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Memory

Teenager with hyperthymesia exhibits extraordinary mental time travel abilities

September 1, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New research finds the cumulative weight of social hardship across a lifespan shapes the aging brain

U.S. sees 5.7 million more childless women than expected, fueling a “demographic cliff”

AI hate speech detectors show major inconsistencies, new study reveals

New study sheds light on how sexual self-disclosure relates to relationship quality

Brain scan study connects parahippocampal cortex thinning with depression and neuroticism

People experiencing manic episodes have measurably higher skin temperatures

Higher cognitive ability and other psychological factors predict support for free speech

Autistic individuals and those with social anxiety differ in how they experience empathy, new study suggests

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy