Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Repeating the “F” word can improve threshold for pain during an ice water challenge

by Beth Ellwood
May 21, 2020
in Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: Craig Sunter)

(Photo credit: Craig Sunter)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A recent study found that repeating the “F” word during an ice water experiment increased subjects’ tolerance and threshold for pain. However, reciting made-up swear words showed no such pain-reducing effects. The study was published in Frontiers in Psychology.

Numerous studies have shown that the use of swear words can strengthen pain tolerance during an ice water experiment. UK researchers Stephens and Robertson set out to explore the mechanism behind this pain-relieving effect in a unique way. A team of specialists invented two new “swear” words with properties similar to known curse words. They then tested the invented words in a cold pressor experiment to see whether they would mimic the pain-reducing effects of known swear words.

One theory suggests that swearing produces analgesic effects through autonomic arousal caused by increased emotion. To explore this idea, researchers chose the made-up swear word “fouch”, selected for its emotion-provoking potential. Another theory suggests that swearing alleviates pain by distracting attention away from the painful event. Researchers accordingly chose the second made-up curse word “twizpipe”, for its potential to evoke distraction through humor.

A total of 92 individuals with a mean age of 27 took part in an experiment that included four conditions. Subjects were asked to submerge one hand in an ice water bath maintained at 3-5° C and were instructed to repeat a designated word while keeping their hand in the water. Depending on the condition, subjects were allocated either a neutral word (a word used to describe a table), the “F” word, the invented swear word “fouch”, or the invented swear word “twizpipe.” Subjects were asked to report when they begin to feel pain (an indication of pain threshold) and to only remove their hand once the pain became unbearable (an indication of pain tolerance).

Results replicated previous findings showing that repeating the “F” word increased participants’ tolerance for pain. In addition, results demonstrated for the first time that repeating the “F” word also increased subjects’ threshold for pain.

Surprisingly, reciting the made-up swear words of “fouch” and “twizpipe” did not increase pain tolerance nor pain threshold. The authors explain that the lack of pain-alleviating effects from the invented swear words, while unexpected, is not wholly surprising. “While it is not properly understood how swear words gain their power,” the authors say, “it has been suggested that swearing is learned during childhood and that aversive classical conditioning contributes to the emotionally arousing aspects of swear word use.”

While the words “fouch” and “twizpipe” were selected for their ability to evoke emotion and humour similar to traditional swear words, “these properties did not facilitate pain alleviation effects, suggesting that surface properties of swear words (such as how they sound) do not explain the hypoalgesic effects of swearing.”

Researchers also tested whether certain properties of swear words could explain the beneficial effect of swearing on pain tolerance. Following each trial, participants rated the word they had been asked to repeat with respect to the concepts of humor, emotion, and distraction. After mediation analysis, it was found that none of these three properties mediated the effects of swearing on pain tolerance.

Researchers point out the significance of these null findings. “While swearing was rated as distracting (more so than the other words) the level of distraction was not related to the pain alleviation effect.” The authors continue, “distraction may not be important in explaining how swearing produces hypoalgesic effect.”

The researchers conclude that their study provides new evidence that swearing can boost the threshold for pain, but that mediation analysis did not provide insight into the properties of swear words that make analgesic effects possible. The authors address the limitation that their study made no distinction between the type of emotion elicited by the words, and suggest that future research assess both positive and negative emotional arousal when it comes to swearing.

The study, “Swearing as a Response to Pain: Assessing Hypoalgesic Effects of Novel “Swear” Words”, was authored by Richard Stevens and Olly Robertson.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin2ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to more prone to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails
Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

Inside the bored brain: Unlocking the power of the default mode network

Choline imbalance in the brain linked to with cognitive symptoms in young depression patients

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

Sedentary time linked to faster brain aging in older adults, study finds

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

Being adopted doesn’t change how teens handle love and dating

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy