Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Repeating things aloud to another person boosts memory recall, study finds

by University of Montreal
October 6, 2015
in Cognitive Science
Photo credit: Université de Montréal

Photo credit: Université de Montréal

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Repeating aloud boosts verbal memory, especially when you do it while addressing another person, says Professor Victor Boucher of the University of Montreal’s Department of Linguistics and Translation. His findings are the result of a study that will be published in the next edition of Consciousness and Cognition.

“We knew that repeating aloud was good for memory, but this is the first study to show that if it is done in a context of communication, the effect is greater in terms of information recall,” Boucher explained.

To demonstrate this, Boucher and Alexis Lafleur asked 44 French-speaking university students to read a series of lexemes on a screen. A lexeme is a word such as it is found in a dictionary. During the task, the participants wore headphones that emitted “white noise” to mask their own voices and eliminate auditory feedback. The subjects were submitted to four experimental conditions: repeating in their head, repeating silently while moving their lips, repeating aloud while looking at the screen, and finally, repeating aloud while addressing someone. After a distraction task, they were asked to identify the lexemes they recalled having said from a list that included lexemes not used in the test.

The results show a clear difference when the exercise was performed aloud in the presence of someone else, even though the participants had heard absolutely nothing. Repeating in one’s head without gesturing was the least effective way to recall information. “The simple fact of articulating without making a sound creates a sensorimotor link that increases our ability to remember, but if it is related to the functionality of speech, we remember even more,” Boucher said.

Previous studies conducted at Professor Boucher’s Phonetic Sciences Laboratory have shown that when we articulate a sound, we create a sensory and motor reference in our brain, by moving our mouth and feeling our vocal chords vibrate. “The production of one or more sensory aspects allows for more efficient recall of the verbal element. But the added effect of talking to someone shows that in addition to the sensorimotor aspects related to verbal expression, the brain refers to the multisensory information associated with the communication episode,” Boucher explained. “The result is that the information is better retained in memory.”

Evoking one’s memory of sensory episodes is in part the phenomenon to which French writer Marcel Proust alluded when he referred to “the madeleines of his childhood.” The texture and flavour of these little cakes rekindled in him an emotional connection that reminded him of his mother. But what do we keep in memory? How does episodic and multisensory memory work? These questions are at the heart of Professor Boucher’s work. Challenging the formal approaches in linguistics, particularly the analysis of spoken language through writing, he has endeavoured for several years to build bridges between his discipline and neuroscience.

Lafleur, a former student who is now a doctoral student in neuropsychology, and Boucher conducted another experiment. “This time, we used sequences of syllables that do not form lexemes in French, i.e., non-words,” said the professor. As the researchers expected, their data showed no difference between the various experimental conditions. Subjects did not recall the sequences of “non-words” any better – whether they produced them aloud, silently, or when speaking to someone.

According to the professor, the fact that the information cannot be grafted to verbal elements in memory and involving a sensory reference explains the absence of effects between the conditions of production. “The results of our research confirm the importance of motor sensory experiences in memory retention and help to better define sensory episodes associated with verbal expression,” Boucher concluded.

RELATED

“A concerning finding”: New study on fake news detection uncovers a disturbing trend
Cognitive Science

New study links cognitive style to health misinformation detection

September 15, 2025
Psychology researchers identify a “burnout to extremism” pipeline
Cognitive Science

Cognitive ability becomes increasingly stable after age 3, study finds

September 15, 2025
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Cognitive Science

The origin of the mental number line may be biological, not cultural, according to a new study

September 11, 2025
Childhood adversity may blunt brain development rather than speed it up
Cognitive Science

Biological aging predicts midlife cognitive decline, especially in those raised in poverty

September 8, 2025
Extraverts show faster, stronger, and more patterned emotional reactions
Cognitive Science

Overconfidence in bullshit detection linked to cognitive blind spots and narcissistic traits

September 5, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Cognitive Science

Simple rhythmic sounds can reshape the brain’s entire network landscape, study finds

September 2, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Memory

Teenager with hyperthymesia exhibits extraordinary mental time travel abilities

September 1, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Artificial Intelligence

AI-powered brain stimulation system can boost focus for those who struggle with attention

September 1, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New paper unpacks how Trump uses “strategic victimhood” to justify retaliation

Personalized brain stimulation shows promise as fast-acting depression treatment

A new window into Alzheimer’s: Brain inflammation marker detected years before symptoms appear

New evidence suggests brain’s opioid system helps mediate ketamine’s antidepressant effects

New study links cognitive style to health misinformation detection

Psychology researchers identify a “burnout to extremism” pipeline

Cognitive ability becomes increasingly stable after age 3, study finds

Cannabis compounds have distinct effects on brain connectivity and blood flow, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy