Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Sharing false political information is associated with heightened schizotypy

by Eric W. Dolan
July 25, 2024
in Political Psychology
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A recent study published in the open-access journal PLOS ONE explores how personality traits influence the sharing of false political information on social media. The research reveals that positive schizotypy — a set of traits including paranoia, suspicion, and disrupted thinking patterns — may play a significant role in both accidental and deliberate sharing of misinformation.

Misinformation and disinformation are pervasive online and can result in severe consequences such as political unrest, diminished trust in genuine news, and the proliferation of harmful conspiracy theories. By identifying the specific personality traits and motivations that predispose certain individuals to spread false information, the researchers aimed to deepen the understanding of this behavior and contribute to the development of more effective interventions to curb the spread of misinformation.

Previous studies had suggested that individual differences, such as personality traits and cognitive styles, might influence the sharing of false information, but there was no consensus on which traits were most important. Furthermore, motivations for sharing false information were underexplored. By examining both personality traits (like positive schizotypy) and various motivations, the researchers sought to create a comprehensive framework that could explain why certain individuals are more likely to share false information.

To this end, they conducted a series of four separate studies.

In the first study, the researchers conducted a preregistered cross-sectional online survey to examine the extent to which various individual differences predict the sharing of false information, both accidentally and deliberately. Data collection was carried out using the Qualtrics platform, with participants recruited from the Prolific participant panel. The initial sample consisted of 670 individuals, but after excluding incomplete responses and inauthentic data, the final sample included 614 participants, predominantly women, with an average age of 30.

Participants completed several background measures, including age, gender, education, country of residency, occupational status, and political ideology. Social media usage was assessed through frequency of use, trust in political information, and the extent of political information sharing. The primary variables of interest were the accidental and deliberate sharing of false information, assessed through two specific questions about whether participants had shared political news stories they later found to be false or knew were false at the time of sharing.

The results indicated that cognitive perceptual schizotypy was significantly associated with accidental sharing of false information. Higher levels of schizotypy were linked to a greater likelihood of reporting having shared false information inadvertently. Additionally, a general tendency to share political information online was associated with accidental sharing. However, need for chaos did not predict deliberate sharing of false information, contradicting initial hypotheses.

Building on the findings of Study 1, the second study aimed to include motivational factors in predicting the sharing of false information. This study also used a cross-sectional online survey, again with participants recruited from Prolific. The final sample included 562 U.S. residents with an equal number of participants identifying as Democrat and Republican.

Participants completed similar background and social media measures as in Study 1. Additionally, they answered questions about their motivations for sharing political information, using an 18-item questionnaire that covered six motivational clusters: Prosocial Activism, Attack or Manipulation of Others, Entertainment, Awareness, Political Self-expression, and Fighting False Information.

Participants were asked about their accidental and deliberate sharing of false information, similar to Study 1. Those who reported sharing false information were further queried on their motivations for sharing the specific false story.

The findings revealed that motivations significantly influenced the sharing of false information. The motivation to attack or manipulate others was associated with deliberate sharing of false information. The desire to entertain was linked to accidental sharing, indicating that some people may share false stories because they find them amusing, only to later discover their inaccuracy. The motivation to raise awareness was also significant, suggesting that individuals who believe in spreading information to counter media biases or alert others about perceived threats were more likely to share false information.

Study 3 shifted from self-reports of past behavior to a scenario-based methodology to assess participants’ likelihood of sharing false information. This study again used a cross-sectional online survey, targeting a specific sample of Prolific users who voted for Donald Trump in the 2020 U.S. presidential election. The final sample included 627 participants.

Participants first completed demographic and social media measures, followed by a task where they rated their likelihood of sharing ten political stories (five true and five false). These stories were right-leaning headlines previously rated for partisanship. Participants indicated their willingness to share each story and their perception of its truthfulness.

The results showed that positive schizotypy was associated with a higher likelihood of sharing false stories. This association was modest but significant. Motivations such as raising awareness and sharing for entertainment purposes were also significant predictors of the likelihood of sharing false stories. However, when controlling for other variables, the attack or manipulation motive did not significantly predict sharing behavior, suggesting that its influence might be mediated by other factors.

The fourth study aimed to observe real-world behavior by analyzing participants’ Twitter activity. This two-stage study began with a pre-selection phase involving 2,999 participants who reported sharing political material on social media and having an active, publicly visible Twitter account. Of these, 134 participants completed the follow-up study, with 113 providing usable data.

Participants completed the same background and social media measures as in previous studies, including self-reports of sharing false information. Motivations for sharing political information were assessed using the 18-item questionnaire from earlier studies. Cognitive Perceptual Schizotypy was again measured with the SPQ-BRU.

The researchers then analyzed the participants’ 100 most recent retweets and quote-retweets on Twitter, coding for the presence of false political information based on a database of fact-checked content. The main variable of interest was the number of false posts retweeted without debunking information.

The findings indicated that a minority of participants had shared false stories. Those who did tended to score higher on the magical thinking subscale of schizotypy, suggesting a link between this trait and real-life sharing of false information. Additionally, motivations such as raising awareness and prosocial activism were associated with actual sharing behavior. However, the attack or manipulation motive did not significantly predict real-life sharing, aligning with the findings from Study 3.

“In summary, our four studies provide evidence that positive schizotypy is associated with measures of sharing false political information,” the researchers wrote. “It emerges as more important than any of the personality, cognitive style, or other individual differences we considered. They also provide evidence for the importance of motivation. While a range of motivations for sharing political information online were associated with sharing false information, two appeared particularly important: a desire to share political stories to attack or manipulate others, and to share political stories in order to raise awareness.”

“While individuals reported different motivations for sharing specific false stories, these two factors appeared to influence both deliberate and accidental sharing of false stories. However, it is possible that the Attack motive is associated more with views about whether it is acceptable to share false information, or increased reporting of doing so, rather than the behavior itself. Understanding the role of motivation in more detail, as well as the effects of positive schizotypy, are likely to be productive themes for future misinformation research.”

The study, “Individual differences in sharing false political information on social media: Deliberate and accidental sharing, motivations and positive schizotypy,” was authored by Tom Buchanan, Rotem Perach, Deborah Husbands, Amber F. Tout, Ekaterina Kostyuk, James Kempley, and Laura Joyner.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes
Narcissism

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

June 13, 2025

New research published in Behavioral Sciences shows that certain narcissistic traits are linked to anti-immigrant attitudes through competitive worldviews and ideological beliefs, highlighting the role of personality in shaping how people view immigrants and social hierarchies.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation

New study reveals how MDMA rewires serotonin and oxytocin systems in the brain

Ghosting and ‘breadcrumbing’: the psychological impact of our bad behaviour on dating apps

Older adults who feel criticized by loved ones are more likely to develop depression

New study exposes gap between ADHD drug use and safety research in children

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

Love addiction linked to memory and attention problems

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy