Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Study: Gamers ‘shielded’ from perceptual interference, enhancing their learning abilities

by Garrett Ray Harriman
June 18, 2015
in Cognitive Science
Photo credit: Kelly Hunter (Creative Commons)

Photo credit: Kelly Hunter (Creative Commons)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Research surrounding video games is often controversial, but a recent study shows the positive role gamers’ perceptual strengths have on their learning ability.

Researchers from Brown University’s Laboratory for Cognitive and Perceptual Learning have published findings in PLOS ONE suggesting people who play video games on a regular basis are not only better and faster perceptual (visual) learners than non-frequent gamers, but are more resistant to perceptual interference, which may lead to more stable, long-term learning.

These findings are in good company. Video games have been the focus of much popular and psychological interest in recent years. Repeated studies have shown that “gamers” possess perceptual and attentional skills superior to non-gamers. For instance, gamers more easily differentiate between multiple distracting stimuli and expand their field of attention with greater ease.

Authors Berard, Cain, Watanabe, and Sasaki’s new study plants another flag on the face of video game research. Specifically, they asked if gamers’ heightened attentional abilities also “shield” them from perceptual interference and distractions, and if this shielding, in turn, enhances their long-term perceptual learning.

They recruited 9 gamers (those playing 5 hours or more/week) and 9 non-gamers (those playing less than 1 hour/week) and put them through two sessions of Task Discrimination Tasks (TDT). These exercises require participants to correctly identify the orientation of a sequence of targets on a screen. The target can appear in any of the screen’s four quadrants or its center, and the test introduces “interference” in the form of different backgrounds of vertical or horizontal lines. The speed and accuracy of responses are then measured. To measure memory consolidation (learning), often a full 24 hours passes between TDTs.

Past TDT research has demonstrated that different types of interference produce different perceptual and learning results. Interrupting a current TDT task with a new interference background, for instance, makes target discrimination more difficult. Once trained on one kind of background (horizontal lines), it becomes harder to learn when a new interference (vertical lines) is introduced. Shortening the time between target and interference screens further disrupts learning and memory processes. Both kinds of interference simulate real-world impediments to learning and were integral to this study.

To test how gamers fared against interference, the researchers administered a specialized TDT over the course of two days. Each session was evenly divided between vertical and horizontal interference, essentially functioning as a major perceptual interruption. On top of this, the targets and interference screens switched quicker and quicker (180 milliseconds to 60 milliseconds). Gamer and non-gamer participants took identical tests with identical interference shifts both days.

Replicating previous studies, results showed that gamers more quickly and accurately identified targets than non-gamers as measured by the increased presentation speed of targets and interference. The researchers were further rewarded with data suggesting gamers are more resistant to interference changes than non-gamers. They could recall, with greater accuracy than non-gamers, the position of targets through interjecting interference a full day later.

All of this suggests that gamers, possibly due to their increased exposure and practice with rapid, competing stimuli, reap long-term learning consolidation benefits in the face of interruptive stimuli. The same mechanisms that help them interpret and discriminate a large amount of speedy material may also help their long-term memories consolidate information.

“It may be possible that the vast amount of visual training frequent gamers receive over the years could help contribute to honing consolidation mechanisms in the brain, especially for visually developed skills,” the researchers explained. “Essentially, this would mean that over the 24-hour period of time between the experimental sessions, more efficient consolidation mechanisms could have been operating in the frequent gamers compared to the non-gamers, resulting in better overall learning.”

The authors of the study believe this vein of research could lead to a new model of visual learning, one “offering insight into how frequent gaming affects not only how we deal with presented information, but also how we retain this information, as well.”

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity
Cognitive Science

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity

May 31, 2025

A new study shows that 36 hours without sleep impairs table tennis players’ reaction times, attention, and brain connectivity. The findings reveal how acute sleep deprivation disrupts spatial cognitive processing, with potential consequences for athletic performance and decision-making under pressure.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists pinpoint part of the brain that deciphers memory from new experience
Memory

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

May 31, 2025

Stimulating the brain’s amygdala during memory formation can boost recall after 24 hours, a new study finds. But the effect varies: some people’s memory improves, others’ worsens—and baseline memory performance appears to be the best predictor of outcome.

Read moreDetails
MDMA therapy: Side effects appear mild, but there are problems with the evidence
Cognitive Science

Consciousness remains a mystery after major theory showdown

May 30, 2025

A groundbreaking collaboration has tested two of the most influential theories of consciousness—global neuronal workspace and integrated information theory. While neither came out on top, the project marks a major shift in how scientists approach one of the mind’s biggest mysteries.

Read moreDetails
A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests
Cognitive Science

A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests

May 30, 2025

A recent brain imaging study finds that sucralose, unlike sugar, increases activity in the hypothalamus and boosts hunger, suggesting that calorie-free sweetness may confuse the brain’s appetite control system.

Read moreDetails
Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief
Cognitive Science

Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief

May 28, 2025

People prone to conspiracy theories may share cognitive tendencies with those who experience delusional thinking. Two new studies suggest that biases like anomalous perception and impulsive reasoning help explain why some are more likely to embrace conspiratorial beliefs.

Read moreDetails
Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by
Memory

Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by

May 24, 2025

A new study suggests life feels like it speeds up during periods of personal growth and satisfaction. Rather than routine making time seem short, researchers found that fulfilled, nostalgic memories are more likely to make the past feel like a blur.

Read moreDetails
A colorful brain on a black background
Cognitive Science

Neuroscientists discover how “aha” moments rewire the brain to enhance memory

May 22, 2025

A study using brain scans shows that flashes of insight reorganize neural patterns in the visual cortex and engage memory and emotion regions, helping embed the solution more deeply in long-term memory.

Read moreDetails
Scientists observe lasting cognitive deficits in long COVID patients
Cognitive Science

Therapeutic video game shows promise for post-COVID cognitive recovery

May 20, 2025

A new study finds that a therapeutic video game, AKL-T01, improved task-switching and processing speed in people with post-COVID cognitive deficits. While sustained attention did not improve, participants reported better quality of life and reduced fatigue after six weeks of gameplay.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Testosterone heightens men’s sensitivity to social feedback and reshapes self-esteem

Estrogen curbs fentanyl intake by suppressing brain’s reward response

New psychology research: Feeling politically excluded heightens antisocial tendencies

Here’s what the data says about who actually benefits from DEI

Adults with ADHD face long-term social and economic challenges, study finds — even with medication

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

Mindfulness boosts generosity only for group-oriented individuals

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy