Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Study finds the most racially prejudiced people tend to think that they are less racist than the average person

by Eric W. Dolan
June 8, 2020
in Cognitive Science, Racism and Discrimination, Social Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New research indicates that the Dunning-Kruger effect — meaning the tendency for less competent individuals to overestimate their competence — can be applied to prejudicial attitudes. The study, which appears in Personality and Individual Differences, found that the least egalitarian individuals tended to be those who overestimated their levels of egalitarianism the most.

“I’m generally interested in how prejudice works and in why it’s sometimes difficult to get people to become less prejudiced. This study opened a window into understanding why some people don’t know that they’re prejudiced, and why those people might be particularly difficult to reach,” said study author Keon West, an associate professor of social psychology and the director of Equalab at Goldsmiths, University of London.

The researchers examined who was the most likely to overestimate their level of egalitarianism in two studies with 307 participants.

Participants were asked to report how egalitarian they believed they were in regards to race (in the first study) or gender (in the second study) compared to the other people. They then stated their favorability towards Black people or women in the workplace. The participants also completed Implicit Association Tests (IATs), which measured implicit biases related to race or gender.

The researchers found that those who reported feeling the most unfavorable toward Black people and displayed the most implicit bias toward Black people were also the ones who most strongly overestimated their racial egalitarianism. Similarly, those who reported feeling the most unfavorable toward women in the workplace and displayed the most implicit bias toward women were also the ones who most strongly overestimated their gender-based egalitarianism.

“You don’t know how prejudiced you are. The most egalitarian people in any given group (i.e., the least prejudiced) tend to think of themselves as above-average (or less prejudiced than the average person in the group),” West told PsyPost.

“But unfortunately, the least egalitarian people in the group also think they’re above average. In other words, even the most prejudiced people tend to think that they are less prejudiced than the average. Do you think you’re less prejudiced than the average person? Probably. But that tells you nothing about where you actually stand.”

“The only way to know is some external or objective measure. Do a few IATs or let an independent party look for measurable signs of prejudiced or egalitarian behaviour. If you don’t do that, you’ll never know,” West said.

Similar research has found that people who perform the worst on a political quiz are the most likely to overestimate their political knowledge. People who strongly endorse misinformation about the link between autism and vaccines, meanwhile, are the most likely to believe that they know as much or more than medical doctors about the causes of autism.

The new research provides evidence that the Dunning-Kruger effect occurs for prejudicial attitudes as well. But it is still unclear why prejudiced people are more likely to misjudge their own prejudice.

“We don’t yet know why this effect occurs. Though we do have a few educated guesses based on similar work in other areas. People who overestimate themselves (i.e., think they’re less prejudiced than they are) probably have very simplistic ideas of what prejudice means (e.g., restricted only to the most obvious and aggressive types) or overestimate how prejudiced other people are. We don’t know, but it’s a good place to start,” West explained.

“Many lay people don’t know that there’s a large and expertly established body of scientific evidence about prejudice. There’s a tendency to reduce it to feelings or opinions about personal lived experience. If you’re one of those people, you’re doing yourself a disservice. There are many things that we know, scientifically, about prejudice. Learning those things, and understanding how we’ve come to know them, is a good first step.”

The study, “Prejudiced and unaware of it: Evidence for the Dunning-Kruger model in the domains of racism and sexism“, was authored by Keon West and Asia A. Eaton.

(Photo credit: Stephen Melkisethian)

TweetSendScanShareSendPin24ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Scientists observe reduced emotional distress in children living near greenery
Racism and Discrimination

Adults’ beliefs about children and race shift when a child’s race is specified, study finds

June 21, 2025

Specifying a child’s race alters how adults perceive their awareness of race and racism, according to new research. Black children are viewed as less “color-evasive” and more racially aware at earlier ages than White children or generic “children.”

Read moreDetails
Study helps untangle the complicated relationship between psychopathy and emotional awareness
Psychopathy

Psychopathic individuals recognize unfairness but are less likely to punish it

June 21, 2025

A new study shows that individuals with higher psychopathic traits are less likely to punish unfair behavior, especially when it costs them personally. The research suggests self-interest, not a lack of moral understanding, drives their reluctance to enforce social norms.

Read moreDetails
The neuroscience of why we cry happy tears
Social Psychology

The neuroscience of why we cry happy tears

June 20, 2025

Why do people cry happy tears? Neuroscience suggests these emotional outbursts occur when the brain becomes overwhelmed by joy, nostalgia, or relief. Far from being irrational, crying during joyful moments helps restore balance and deepen human connection.

Read moreDetails
The fading affect bias impacts most memories — but election-related memories are surprisingly resilient
Memory

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that forgiving someone does not make us forget what they did—but it does change how we feel about it. People who forgave recalled past wrongs with just as much detail, but with less emotional pain.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Psychology researchers find that a simple “talking to strangers” intervention is surprisingly effective
Social Psychology

Simple social rituals like eye contact and small talk are psychologically powerful

June 19, 2025

A social psychology professor highlights how our daily habits—like wearing headphones or avoiding eye contact—can weaken social bonds. Even brief interactions with strangers can boost well-being, but only if we choose to engage rather than withdraw.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Scientists reveal a surprising link between depression and microbes in your mouth

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

Experienced FPS gamers show faster, more efficient eye movements during aiming tasks, study finds

Study links moderate awe in psychedelic ayahuasca journeys to better well-being

Dementia: Tactile decline may signal early cognitive impairment

Adults’ beliefs about children and race shift when a child’s race is specified, study finds

Anxiety and anger may explain how parenting styles shape life satisfaction

Psychopathic individuals recognize unfairness but are less likely to punish it

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy