Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Racism and Discrimination

Adults’ beliefs about children and race shift when a child’s race is specified, study finds

by Eric W. Dolan
June 21, 2025
in Racism and Discrimination
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

New research published in the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin shows that adults’ beliefs about how children perceive and engage with race are influenced by whether the child’s race is explicitly mentioned. When a child’s race was specified as either Black or White, adults were more likely to believe that child noticed racial differences and developed race-related understandings earlier. Black children, in particular, were consistently viewed as developing these capacities earlier than White children or children whose race was not mentioned.

The study was designed to better understand how race influences adults’ assumptions about childhood development in the context of a racially diverse society. Many adults in the United States adopt a “color-evasive” stance when thinking about children, assuming that young people don’t notice race and don’t need to discuss it. The researchers aimed to examine whether naming a child’s race disrupts this default assumption and prompts more nuanced thinking about how children perceive race—and when adults believe it’s appropriate to talk with them about it.

The research team conducted five pre-registered studies. Across these studies, more than 5,000 participants were recruited through online platforms such as TurkPrime, Prolific, and Connect. All participants were adults living in the United States, fluent in English, and completed a series of measures designed to assess their beliefs about when children begin to develop race-related capacities and how color-evasive they perceive children to be. Some studies also included an experimental intervention and open-ended questions to assess how participants would talk to children about race.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of several conditions in which they were asked about either “children” in general or children specified as Black or White. In some studies, participants were shown a brief educational paragraph explaining that children notice and think about racial differences at an early age. The researchers used sliding scales to measure the ages at which participants believed children first show racial preferences, categorize people by race, or associate race with social status. They also asked when it was appropriate to begin discussing race with children, and in one study, had participants respond to a hypothetical question from a child about why most U.S. presidents have been White men.

The findings revealed two consistent patterns. First, adults believed that “children” (when the race was unspecified) were more color-evasive—that is, less likely to notice or think about racial differences—than children identified as Black or White. This suggests that generic language like “children” may prompt race-neutral thinking, in which adults don’t consider children’s lived experiences in a racialized society.

Second, when race was specified, especially when the child was Black, adults believed that children began to understand race at younger ages. On average, Black children were seen as developing race-related capacities about seven months earlier than White children or unspecified children.

These perceptions had consequences. The earlier adults believed a child developed race-related capacities, the younger the age they reported being willing to talk to that child about race. In other words, the assumption that Black children understand race earlier led adults to say they would talk to them about racism at younger ages. This belief did not appear to apply as strongly to White children or to children in general. Additionally, when adults believed that children were more color-evasive, they were less likely to say they would talk to them about race at a young age.

One study included a brief intervention to test whether these beliefs could be shifted. Participants who read a short paragraph explaining that children are naturally aware of race subsequently reported lower beliefs in color-evasiveness across all child groups and said they would be willing to start conversations about race at earlier ages. This suggests that targeted information can change adult beliefs and potentially support more proactive conversations about race with children.

In an exploratory part of the research, participants were asked how they would respond to a child who asked why nearly all U.S. presidents have been White men. Their answers were coded for whether they addressed racism directly or minimized it. Adults were more likely to minimize racism in their responses when the question came from a White child, compared to when it came from a Black child or a child with no specified race.

The more participants believed children understood race, the more they addressed racism directly. Conversely, the more color-evasive they believed children were, the less likely they were to acknowledge racism in their responses.

Notably, adults tended to view the generic category of “children” as White and race-neutral. This aligns with previous research showing that dominant social categories (like “Americans” or “humans”) are often unconsciously imagined as White unless otherwise specified. This mental default may contribute to a lack of engagement with racial issues in conversations with children when their race is not mentioned.

The study also included exploratory analyses comparing responses from White participants and participants of color. White participants tended to believe that children—regardless of racial identity—developed race-related capacities earlier than did participants of color. White participants also reported being more willing to talk about race at younger ages. The authors suggest this might reflect increased awareness or performative intentions in response to recent national conversations about racism, but they emphasize the need for further research to understand these differences more fully.

All five studies in the research were preregistered, meaning the researchers publicly documented their hypotheses, study designs, and planned analyses before collecting data. Preregistration is an important practice in psychological science because it helps prevent questionable research practices, such as selectively reporting results or modifying hypotheses after seeing the data. By committing in advance to specific methods and analyses, researchers increase the transparency and credibility of their findings.

But the researchers acknowledge some limitations. While the sample sizes were large, the participant pool was predominantly White, which may have influenced the results. The study also focused on only two racial groups—Black and White children—limiting the generalizability of the findings to other racial and ethnic backgrounds. Preliminary data from an exploratory study that included Asian, Latino, Indigenous, and biracial children suggested similar trends, but more research is needed to draw firm conclusions.

Future studies could explore how adults think about race in relation to other marginalized or mixed-race groups, how beliefs vary across different social and educational settings, and whether these beliefs change over time. Researchers are also interested in whether adults’ assumptions about children’s race-related understanding influence actual parenting, teaching, or policymaking practices.

The study, “U.S. Adults Believe That “Children” Are Color-Evasive and “Black Children” Process Race Early,” was authored by Leigh S. Wilton, Jess Sullivan, and Evan P. Apfelbaum.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Racism and Discrimination

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

July 3, 2025

Hispanic adolescents reported later sleep timing, greater social jet lag, shorter weekday sleep, higher caffeine intake, and more evening screen time than peers. Screen use and stress, but not caffeine, were linked to delayed sleep and sleep misalignment.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Some dark personality traits may help buffer against depression, new psychology research suggests
COVID-19

COVID-19 coverage linked to rise in anti-Asian sentiment, especially among Trump supporters

June 17, 2025

Americans became less favorable toward Asians as COVID-19 spread and news coverage intensified, according to a new study. The drop was strongest among Trump supporters, highlighting how political rhetoric and fear shaped public opinion during the pandemic.

Read moreDetails
New research identifies two belief systems that shape reactions to diversity initiatives
Racism and Discrimination

White Americans do not feel threatened by demographic change, new study finds

June 10, 2025

A new study casts doubt on the idea that White Americans respond negatively to news about becoming a demographic minority, finding little evidence that such messages influence political views, racial attitudes, or feelings of threat across ideological lines.

Read moreDetails
Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children
Political Psychology

New research sheds light on growing support for Black candidates among white Democrats

June 2, 2025

New research shows that white Democrats are increasingly choosing Black candidates over white ones in hypothetical elections. The change reflects a broader shift in racial attitudes, especially among those concerned about discrimination and opposed to Donald Trump’s rhetoric.

Read moreDetails
Here’s what the data says about who actually benefits from DEI
Business

Here’s what the data says about who actually benefits from DEI

May 31, 2025

What’s the actual impact of diversity, equity, and inclusion? A sociologist unpacks decades of research showing how DEI programs affect businesses, education, and the broader economy—highlighting who benefits, who doesn’t, and what the data really says.

Read moreDetails
Study finds Americans overwhelmingly believe sports promote racial integration—but reality is more complex
Racism and Discrimination

Study finds Americans overwhelmingly believe sports promote racial integration—but reality is more complex

May 2, 2025

Do sports really bring people together? According to a national survey, most Americans think so—especially those who’ve played on racially diverse teams.

Read moreDetails
Friendships across gender and race linked to greater kindness in children, study finds
Developmental Psychology

Friendships across gender and race linked to greater kindness in children, study finds

April 29, 2025

New research suggests that diverse childhood friendships—especially those across gender—can encourage kindness not just within friendships, but toward peers from other social groups.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Scientists discover weak Dems have highest testosterone — but there’s an intriguing twist

Can sunshine make you happier? A massive study offers a surprising answer

New study links why people use pornography to day-to-day couple behavior

Virtual reality meditation eases caregiver anxiety during pediatric hospital stays, with stronger benefits for Spanish speakers

Fascinating new advances in psychedelic science reveal how they may heal the mind

Dysfunction within the sensory processing cortex of the brain is associated with insomnia, study finds

Prenatal exposure to “forever chemicals” linked to autistic traits in children, study finds

Ketamine repairs reward circuitry to reverse stress-induced anhedonia

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy