Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Mental Health Body Image and Body Dysmorphia

Can being objectified affect women’s cognitive ability? New study has a surprising answer

by Eric W. Dolan
January 24, 2024
in Body Image and Body Dysmorphia, Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

New research suggests that self-objectification—often thought to impair cognitive performance in women—might not have the detrimental effects previously assumed. The study explored the impact of self-objectification and the anticipation of a sexualized male gaze on cognitive abilities, particularly working memory. Contrary to widespread belief, the study revealed no significant impairment in cognitive performance due to self-objectification. The findings have been published in PLOS One.

The motivation for this research emerged from a longstanding debate in psychological science. Prevailing theories, like Objectification Theory proposed by Fredrickson and Roberts in 1997, suggest that when women are objectified — treated as objects for others’ use and pleasure — they may start self-objectifying, leading to a host of negative psychological outcomes.

One of the key claims of this theory is that self-objectification consumes women’s attention, diverting it towards body monitoring and potentially impairing performance in tasks requiring cognitive resources. Previous studies had produced inconsistent results, prompting the Northwestern University researchers to undertake a more robust investigation.

“We are fascinated by empirical research that has clear implications for women’s everyday lives,” explained Renee Engeln, a professor of instruction in psychology and the author of “Beauty Sick: How the Cultural Obsession with Appearance Hurts Girls and Women.”

“We’re also invested in social psychology’s growing emphasis on well-powered, replicable research. This study was a chance to conduct a rigorous test of the extent to which being objectified can affect women’s cognitive performance. Given how often women experience sexual objectification, if these experiences are affecting women’s ability to concentrate, it’s important to be able to document and understand that phenomenon.”

To explore this, Engeln and her co-author Anne Zola designed a well-powered experiment with 407 women initially participating, with 376 remaining after exclusions for various reasons. The participants, aged 18–25, were predominantly college students.

Participants were randomly divided into three distinct groups: a control group, a self-objectification group, and a male gaze group. In the control group, participants’ cognitive performance was assessed in a standard, non-objectifying environment. The self-objectification group, on the other hand, experienced a heightened awareness of their bodies, as they were video-recorded from the neck down. The third group, the male gaze group, was not only recorded but also led to believe that these recordings would later be assessed by men in a dating study context.

The centerpiece of the study was a working memory task, which was modeled after a subsection of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV. This task was specifically chosen for its ability to assess attention and working memory without the need for advanced mathematical skills, focusing instead on simple arithmetic problems that required mental computation. This approach was deliberately chosen to avoid confounding factors like math anxiety or stereotype threat – a phenomenon where individuals perform poorly on tasks where their group is stereotypically expected to do so.

Before commencing the working memory task, each participant completed pre-tests measuring their current level of anxiety and guilt. They were then placed in the conditions specific to their group, with the self-objectification and male gaze groups being video-recorded. The working memory task involved listening to, processing, and mentally solving arithmetic problems, then writing the final answers on a whiteboard.

After completing the task, participants were given post-tests mirroring the pre-tests to measure any changes in anxiety, guilt, and body image state. They were also asked to guess the study’s hypothesis to check if their guesses influenced the outcomes. Furthermore, the Objectified Body Consciousness Scale was adapted to measure the extent to which participants were preoccupied with monitoring their physical appearance – a key indicator of state self-objectification.

Contrary to expectations, the results showed that while the video-recording did increase self-objectification, it did not lead to a reduction in cognitive performance on working memory tasks. There was no significant difference in the accuracy of responses or the time taken to complete problems among the three groups. Furthermore, no association was found between state body surveillance scores and either response accuracy or latency. This suggests that the level of body surveillance experienced by participants did not impact their cognitive performance.

“We should all remember that a single study doesn’t ‘prove’ (or disprove!) a scientific claim. It’s tempting to simplify the stories we tell about research findings, but the deeper you dig, the more complexity and nuance you often find,” Engeln told PsyPost.

“We know that attention is a limited resource. And it makes sense that if you’re focused on the fact that others are evaluating how your body looks, that’s going to claim part of your attention. We found that when objectified, the women in our study focused more on how they looked. But we didn’t find that being objectified had a measurable effect on women’s working memory. Most women have likely had a lot of practice staying focused on a task even when someone is evaluating their physical appearance. Research on this topic may have overlooked women’s resilience in response to these experiences.”

Interestingly, the study also found that completing the working memory task increased anxiety and guilt across all conditions, indicating that the task itself, regardless of the condition, made participants feel worse. However, the change was not different across the experimental conditions.

“We were surprised at how unpleasant our participants found the experience of having to answer relatively simple math problems out loud,” Engeln said. “Math anxiety wasn’t the focus of our study, but it’s certainly a reminder that even very academically talented women like those in this study can still experience substantial anxiety about their abilities.”

While the study’s methodology was robust, including a well-powered design and preregistration, it did have limitations. The sample was narrow, consisting mostly of college students from a highly selective university, which might not represent the broader population. The study also did not control for individual differences like sexual orientation or trait self-objectification, which could influence responses to objectification. Moreover, the study’s focus on a specific demographic and setting limits the generalizability of the findings.

Given the surprising results of this study, further exploration into the conditions under which objectification might impact cognitive performance is needed. Studies could consider a wider array of cognitive tasks and include a more diverse participant pool to understand the role of individual differences in these effects. Additionally, understanding how women combat objectifying environments and thoughts could provide new insights into resilience in the face of objectification.

“Researchers have tried to create ‘objectification’ in a lab setting in many different ways,” Engeln explained. “There is still much to learn in terms of understanding whether some types of objectification might have bigger effects on women’s cognition than others.”

“For example, women may respond differently when their appearance is evaluated in a non-sexual way (as it might be during a job interview, for example) compared to when they are openly sexually objectified (like when a woman is cat-called). We also know very little about how women cope with being objectified or how they minimize the effects of being objectified. That’s an important area for future research.”

The study, “Brains over beauty: A preregistered test of the effects of objectification on women’s cognitive performance“, was published September 21, 2023.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to more prone to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails
Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

Inside the bored brain: Unlocking the power of the default mode network

Choline imbalance in the brain linked to with cognitive symptoms in young depression patients

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

Sedentary time linked to faster brain aging in older adults, study finds

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

Being adopted doesn’t change how teens handle love and dating

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy