Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

New psychology research adds another twist to Stanley Milgram’s famous obedience findings

by Eric W. Dolan
February 1, 2024
in Social Psychology
A participant in Stanley Milgram's experiment, featured in the 1962 documentary "Obedience."

A participant in Stanley Milgram's experiment, featured in the 1962 documentary "Obedience."

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

In a series of studies inspired by Stanley Milgram’s obedience experiments, researchers explored how the perceived status of scientific disciplines influences people’s willingness to obey authority. They discovered that participants were more likely to follow instructions believed to advance “hard” sciences like neuroscience compared to “soft” sciences like social science. The research, published in the British Journal of Social Psychology, sheds light on the role of perceptions of science in shaping obedience.

“I think most of us have been in positions, whether at work or school, where we’ve felt pressure to do something, or go along with something, that we’re not comfortable with,” said study author Megan Birney, a senior lecturer at Staffordshire University and the author of “Self & Identity: The Basics”

“Our research shows that going along with others’ requests to do something is not blind or without thought, but rather, a process of identity. For me, it’s important that we gain a deeper understanding of this so that we can hold ourselves, as well as others, accountable.”

Background: The Psychology of Obedience to Authority

In the 1960s, psychologist Stanley Milgram conducted a series of experiments that have since become famous for their striking insights into the power of authority over individual behavior. These experiments, known as the “Obedience to Authority” studies, were designed to understand how far individuals would go in obeying an authority figure, especially when their actions conflicted with their personal conscience and ethical beliefs.

In these experiments, participants were assigned the role of a ‘Teacher’ and were instructed to administer electric shocks to another participant, the ‘Learner’, every time the Learner made an error on a memory task. Unbeknownst to the Teacher, the Learner was actually an actor, and no real shocks were given. As the experiment progressed, the shocks increased in intensity, eventually reaching levels labeled as dangerous.

The pivotal finding of Milgram’s study was that a significant majority of the Teachers, despite showing signs of stress and discomfort, continued to administer shocks up to the highest levels, purely because they were urged to do so by the experiment’s authority figure, the Experimenter.

These experiments shed light on the disturbing extent to which individuals might conform to authority, even when it involves harming others, a revelation that was particularly poignant in the context of the atrocities committed during World War II. Milgram’s work has since become a cornerstone in the study of social psychology, illustrating the often underestimated power of authority figures in shaping individual behavior.

Milgram initially concluded that people enter an “agentic state” under authority, losing awareness of the consequences of their actions. However, this explanation has been increasingly challenged. Recent interpretations, such as the “engaged followership” account, suggest that participants’ actions in Milgram’s experiments were more about their identification with the scientific project and less about blind obedience to authority.

The researchers’ current study builds on this alternative interpretation. They aimed to understand how the perception of a scientific endeavor as “prototypical” (or representative of ideal science) influences participants’ willingness to follow experimental instructions Specifically, they sought to understand if the distinction between so-called “hard” sciences and “soft” sciences could affect people’s obedience to scientific authority. They conducted a series of four studies.

Study 1: Initial Test of Neuroscience vs. Social Science

In the first study, researchers sought to understand how the perceived prototypicality of different scientific disciplines might influence participants’ obedience. They recruited 198 postgraduate students from various disciplines and designed an online task modeled after Milgram’s obedience experiments. However, instead of administering electric shocks, participants were asked to assign negative descriptors to a range of social groups. The experiment was framed in two different contexts: as advancing neuroscience, considered a ‘hard’ science, and as advancing social science, often perceived as a ‘soft’ science.

The results showed that participants who believed they were contributing to neuroscience were more willing to follow through with the task, completing more trials than those in the social science condition. This suggested that the perception of neuroscience as a more prototypical, or ‘harder’, science could lead to greater obedience, possibly due to increased trust in the discipline.

Study 2: Broadening the Participant Pool

Building on the findings from Study 1, the researchers in Study 2 aimed to see if the same effects would be observed in a more diverse sample. They recruited 396 individuals from the general population through Prolific, an online platform. Similar to the first study, participants were divided into three groups: those advancing neuroscience, those advancing social science, and a control group with no specific scientific discipline mentioned.

The results of Study 2 were mostly consistent with Study 1, though the effects were less pronounced. Participants in the neuroscience condition showed a slightly higher tendency to complete the task than those in the social science condition. However, the difference was not as stark as in the first study, indicating that the effect of scientific discipline on obedience might be more subtle among a general population than among postgraduate students.

Study 3: Context and the COVID-19 Pandemic

In Study 3, the researchers aimed to replicate their previous findings with a new twist. Conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, this study involved 382 participants from the general population. The pandemic’s context, which highlighted the importance of social science, provided a unique backdrop for this research. The study replicated the methodology of the previous experiments but added questions about the participants’ experience with the task.

Surprisingly, Study 3 did not find significant differences in obedience between the neuroscience and social science conditions. This indicated that the specific context and the heightened profile of social science during the pandemic might have influenced participants’ perceptions, making them as likely to obey instructions in a social science context as in a neuroscience one.

Study 4: Exploring Perceptions without Manipulation

The fourth study shifted focus from direct manipulation of scientific discipline to exploring participants’ perceptions about the study and the experimenters. The research involved 306 participants, and, like its predecessors, was conducted online. Participants completed the same task but were not told it was advancing a particular field of science. After the task, they answered questions about their perceptions of the study’s and the experimenter’s prototypicality of science, along with their feelings about participation.

The results from Study 4 revealed that perceptions of the study being prototypical of science were linked to greater trust in researchers, a sense of the study’s worthiness, happiness about participation, and less dislike for the task. These factors, in turn, correlated with greater followership or a higher likelihood of completing the task. Interestingly, these effects were more consistently associated with perceptions of the study’s prototypicality than with perceptions of the experimenter’s prototypicality.

Overall Insights

These studies collectively suggest that the perceived prototypicality of scientific disciplines can influence obedience to scientific authority. However, this effect is nuanced and can be influenced by the context, as shown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Study 3.

“There are often competing voices in our head about what we should do when we’re in tricky situations,” Birney told PsyPost. “I think the takeaway from this is that it is worth stopping to reflect on our actions – not just our behaviours, but the pull we felt to act as we’ve done.”

Limitations and Future Research

While these studies provide valuable insights into the influence of obedience to authority, they are not without limitations. The online nature of the tasks may not fully capture the dynamics of in-person authority. Additionally, the context of the COVID-19 pandemic during the third study highlighted how external factors could influence perceptions of scientific disciplines. Future research could delve deeper into these nuances, exploring how different attributes of scientific disciplines influence obedience and the role of personal identification with science in this process.

The study, “Engaged followership and toxic science: Exploring the effect of prototypicality on willingness to follow harmful experimental instructions“, was authored by Megan E. Birney, Stephen D. Reicher, S. Alexander Haslam, Niklas K. Steffens, and Fergus G. Neville.

RELATED

Study uncovers a gendered double standard for interracial relationships
Attachment Styles

Attachment insecurity shapes mentalization in interracial long-distance relationships

September 13, 2025
New study identifies two factors that help explain the link between narcissism and self-esteem
Body Image and Body Dysmorphia

Women prone to self-objectification tend to have lower empathy

September 12, 2025
COVID-19 lockdowns linked to lasting disruptions in teen brain and body systems
Evolutionary Psychology

Large cross-cultural study finds your “body count” affects your desirability, with little evidence of gender bias

September 11, 2025
Scientists identify a mysterious brain signal tied to stress and hormone pulses
Sexism

Students rate identical lectures differently based on professor’s gender, researchers find

September 10, 2025
What people love most about sex, according to new psychology research
Attractiveness

Attraction goes beyond looks: Study shows voices, scents, and motion all matter

September 8, 2025
Artificial intelligence reveals Trump’s language as both uniquely simplistic and divisive among U.S. presidents
Political Psychology

Elite rhetoric about Trump’s prosecution had limited impact on Republican and independent voters

September 7, 2025
Extraverts show faster, stronger, and more patterned emotional reactions
Attractiveness

People interpret long eyelashes as a signal of openness to casual relationships

September 7, 2025
Narcissists, psychopaths, and sadists often believe they are morally superior
Neuroimaging

Antisocial personality traits linked to blunted brain responses to angry faces

September 5, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Cannabidiol shows no immediate effect on brain or behavior in young people with alcohol use disorder, study finds

From brain circuits to gut health, a new review details the complex biology of mood disorders

Attachment insecurity shapes mentalization in interracial long-distance relationships

AI detects hidden movement clues linked to Parkinson’s disease

New research complicates the story of dog domestication

Harvard scientists pinpoint how sleep stabilizes memory in fascinating neuroscience breakthrough

Surprising new findings force scientists to rethink decades of brain-plasticity theories

Breath-based meditation technique shifts brain into deeply relaxed state, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy