Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

People overwhelmingly choose “natural” products, from chocolate to drugs

by Eric W. Dolan
December 16, 2024
in Social Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study sheds light on the deep-seated human preference for all things “natural.” Published in Social Psychological and Personality Science, the research shows that this bias extends beyond words, influencing actual decisions—even when those decisions involve potential risks, like taking a drug or eating food that might cause discomfort.

The researchers behind the new study were intrigued by the well-documented naturalness bias—the widespread preference for items described as natural over synthetic. Prior studies have shown that this bias extends across various domains, from food and medicine to personal care products. However, much of the existing research relied on self-reported preferences in hypothetical scenarios, leaving a gap in understanding how the bias influences actual behavior.

“Most of us love nature. Waterfalls, mountains, forest trails, parks, and other green spaces provide endless enjoyment. Yet, can the love of nature and naturalness in general bias our beliefs in some situations?” said study author Brian Meier, a professor of psychology at Gettysburg College.

“My colleagues and I have been interested for some time in how terms related to ‘nature,’ ‘natural,’ or ‘naturalness’ might bias our perceptions of a host of items. In the present case, we focused on medicines. We have several papers showing that labeling a drug as ‘natural’ versus ‘synthetic’ affects attitudes, safety ratings, and choices in self-reports. Simply put, on average, people think natural drugs are better, safer, and a good choice compared to drugs described as synthetic or human-made.”

“Yet, much of this prior work has been carried out using hypothetical scenarios or situations where people tell researchers what they think they would feel or do,” Meier explained. “Therefore, it is unclear if people would actually use natural versus synthetic items when we measure their observable behavior.”

The researchers conducted four experiments, each designed to test participants’ choices and behaviors in different contexts.

The first experiment focused on a scenario involving a “performance-enhancing” drink. The participants, 174 college students, were informed that the study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of a drink described as either natural or synthetic in improving strength. Participants completed an initial self-control task, where they measured their grip strength by holding a dynamometer at a set pressure. They were then offered the opportunity to try one of the drinks, described as natural or synthetic, before repeating the task.

In reality, both drinks were simply water, but the participants believed they were consuming a functional performance enhancer. The researchers recorded their choices and whether they followed through with drinking it. Remarkably, 84% of the participants opted for the drink labeled as natural, demonstrating a strong preference for naturalness even in this relatively low-risk scenario.

The second experiment introduced a more consequential context by simulating a medical procedure. Fifty-two students were told they could volunteer to test a blood-coagulating drug delivered via a finger prick. The drug was described as either natural or synthetic, but the finger stick itself was a sham; no actual lancet was used, and no drug was administered. To enhance the realism, the procedure was staged with typical medical equipment, such as gloves and alcohol wipes, and carried out by a research assistant wearing a lab coat.

Despite the potential discomfort or risk implied by the procedure, 73% of the participants who agreed to participate chose the natural version of the drug. This result indicated that even when a decision involved physical consequences, the naturalness bias remained a significant factor in participants’ choices.

The third experiment examined how the naturalness bias played out in the context of food, specifically chocolate consumption. Ninety-eight students were offered the chance to taste chocolate that was described as being made with either natural or synthetic cocoa. Participants were told that the cocoa might cause stomach discomfort, adding a layer of potential risk to their decision. After selecting and eating the chocolate, they rated its taste and any discomfort they experienced. Meier and his colleagues found that 84% of the participants who volunteered chose the natural cocoa chocolate.

The fourth and final experiment moved outside the laboratory and into a public setting. It included 200 participants, making it the most diverse of the studies. The researchers approached passersby on and around a university campus and asked them to evaluate and choose between stickers described as being made with either natural or synthetic ink. After rating the stickers for appeal and quality, participants were allowed to keep one as a token of appreciation.

Here, 66% of the participants chose a sticker labeled as natural. Although the stakes in this scenario were low, the preference for natural options was still evident. The participants’ ratings of the stickers also revealed that they perceived the natural ones to be of higher quality and more appealing.

“Most of the participants who completed the studies were biased by the term ‘natural’ in their behavior,” Meier told PsyPost. “That is, around 75-80% of participants in these studies chose a natural versus synthetic item and actually followed through with the behavior in question (e.g., chocolate consumption or drug ‘injection’). These results suggest to us that this naturalness bias can influence behavior that some people might consider risky.”

The researchers propose several future directions for this line of inquiry, including identifying individuals most and least susceptible to the bias, exploring ways to mitigate its influence, and examining how the bias impacts consequential medical behaviors, such as vaccine uptake.

The study, “Perceived Naturalness Biases Objective Behavior in Both Trivial and Meaningful Contexts,” was authored by Brian P. Meier, Eric E. Noreen, Li-Jun Ji, Michael B. Fellman, and Courtney M. Lappas.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Psychology researchers find that a simple “talking to strangers” intervention is surprisingly effective
Social Psychology

Simple social rituals like eye contact and small talk are psychologically powerful

June 19, 2025

A social psychology professor highlights how our daily habits—like wearing headphones or avoiding eye contact—can weaken social bonds. Even brief interactions with strangers can boost well-being, but only if we choose to engage rather than withdraw.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Attractive female students no longer earned higher grades when classes moved online during COVID-19
Social Psychology

Personality stays mostly the same after moving up in social class, new study suggests

June 18, 2025

In a long-term study of German youth, first-generation students who attended university became slightly more risk-averse. However, traits like conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness developed similarly to peers from similar backgrounds who didn’t pursue higher education.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Some dark personality traits may help buffer against depression, new psychology research suggests
COVID-19

COVID-19 coverage linked to rise in anti-Asian sentiment, especially among Trump supporters

June 17, 2025

Americans became less favorable toward Asians as COVID-19 spread and news coverage intensified, according to a new study. The drop was strongest among Trump supporters, highlighting how political rhetoric and fear shaped public opinion during the pandemic.

Read moreDetails
Some dark personality traits may help buffer against depression, new psychology research suggests
Dark Triad

Some dark personality traits may help buffer against depression, new psychology research suggests

June 17, 2025

Dark traits like narcissism and psychopathy are often associated with dysfunction, but new findings reveal that certain facets—especially Machiavellian agency—might help people manage stress and depression more effectively through adaptive coping strategies.

Read moreDetails
Dark personality traits and specific humor styles are linked to online trolling, study finds
Artificial Intelligence

Memes can serve as strong indicators of coming mass violence

June 15, 2025

A new study finds that surges in visual propaganda—like memes and doctored images—often precede political violence. By combining AI with expert analysis, researchers tracked manipulated content leading up to Russia’s invasion of Ukraine, revealing early warning signs of instability.

Read moreDetails
Evolutionary scholars worry political trends are impeding progress in evolutionary psychology, according to new study
Social Psychology

Scientists uncover surprisingly consistent pattern of scholarly curiosity throughout history

June 15, 2025

A sweeping new study of thousands of historical scholars suggests that human curiosity may follow a universal blueprint. Across centuries and civilizations, intellectual interests appear to cluster in surprisingly consistent ways—hinting at deep patterns in how we seek knowledge.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Do AI tools undermine our sense of creativity? New study says yes

Simple social rituals like eye contact and small talk are psychologically powerful

New research links heatwaves to depression and anxiety in adolescents, with some at higher risk

Religious attendance may not boost mental health, long-term study finds

Menstrual cycle hormone levels influence women’s attention to female faces, brain imaging study finds

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

Study links psychedelic use in illegal settings to increased psychotic and manic symptoms

Personality stays mostly the same after moving up in social class, new study suggests

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy