Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

People trust typical-looking faces most

by Association for Psychological Science
December 16, 2014
in Social Psychology
Photo credit: Greg Peverill-Conti (Creative Commons)

Photo credit: Greg Peverill-Conti (Creative Commons)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Being “average” is often considered a bad thing, but new research suggests that averageness wins when people assess the trustworthiness of a face. The research indicates that, while typical-looking faces aren’t seen as the most attractive, they are considered to be the most trustworthy. The new findings are published inPsychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

“Face typicality likely indicates familiarity and cultural affiliation — as such, these findings have important implications for understanding social perception, including cross-cultural perceptions and interactions,” explains psychological scientist and lead researcher Carmel Sofer of Princeton University and Radboud University Nijmegen in the Netherlands.

Previous studies have shown that a face composed as an average of many faces is often perceived as more attractive than the sum of its constituent parts. But other studies suggest that the relationship between averageness and attractiveness may not be so simple, and that some dimensions of faces matter more than others in explaining the connection. Sofer and colleagues wondered whether typicality might be more directly tied to perceptions of trustworthiness.

In one experiment, the researchers created a “typical” face by digitally averaging 92 female faces, and they also created an “attractive” face by averaging the 12 most attractive faces from another set of faces. They then combined the two faces into one and created nine variations that had differing levels of attractiveness and typicality. The final result was a continuum of 11 faces that ranged from least attractive to most attractive, with the most typical face occupying the midpoint.

Female participants viewed these face variations and used a 9-point scale to rate them on either trustworthiness or attractiveness; over the course of the study, the participants saw and rated each face three times. The researchers only included female participants so as to eliminate potential cross-gender differences in how people perceive and evaluate faces.

The resulting ratings revealed a sort of U-shaped relationship between face typicality and trustworthiness: The closer a face was to the most typical face, the more trustworthy it was considered to be.

When it came to attractiveness, however, typicality didn’t seem to play a role — participants rated faces as increasingly more attractive beyond the midpoint of the most typical face.

“Although face typicality did not matter for attractiveness judgments, it mattered a great deal for trustworthiness judgments,” Sofer explains. “This effect may have been overlooked, because trustworthiness and attractiveness judgments are generally highly correlated in research.”

Another experiment confirmed these findings, showing the relationship between averageness and trustworthiness was not driven by the specific faces used or the by the transformation process that the researchers had employed to digitally combine and alter the faces.

“By showing the influence of face typicality on perceived trustworthiness, our findings cast a new light on how face typicality influences social perception,” the researchers write. “They highlight the social meaning of the typical face because trustworthiness judgments approximate the general evaluation of faces.”

Sofer and colleagues are interested in exploring how face typicality influence the face-related judgments we make in cross-cultural environments.

“We are interested in how people judge face trustworthiness when visiting other countries and how the locals perceive the visitors,” Sofer explains. “In addition, we plan to study how face typicality influences trustworthiness judgments, when other factors such as emotional expressions are present.”

Study co-authors include Ron Dotsch of Utrecht University and Radboud University Nijmegen, Daniel H.J. Wigboldus of Radboud University Nijmegen, and Alexander Todorov of Princeton University.

All data and materials have been made publicly available via Open Science Framework and can be accessed athttps://osf.io/nxrsj/. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data.

This article has received badges for Open Data and Open Materials. More information about the Open Practices badges can be found at https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/view/ and http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full.

RELATED

Fetuses show preference for face-like patterns
Parenting

U.S. sees 5.7 million more childless women than expected, fueling a “demographic cliff”

September 17, 2025
Autistic individuals and those with social anxiety differ in how they experience empathy, new study suggests
Political Psychology

Higher cognitive ability and other psychological factors predict support for free speech

September 17, 2025
New study identifies sexual frustration as a significant factor in mass shootings
Racism and Discrimination

New study finds strong links between prejudice and support for political violence in the United States

September 16, 2025
The way you blink reveals how music is shaping your attention, new study finds
Evolutionary Psychology

Women often display more aggression than men toward their siblings, large global study finds

September 16, 2025
Both-sidesism debunked? Study finds conservatives more anti-democratic, driven by two psychological traits
Authoritarianism

New paper unpacks how Trump uses “strategic victimhood” to justify retaliation

September 15, 2025
Psychology researchers identify a “burnout to extremism” pipeline
Business

Psychology researchers identify a “burnout to extremism” pipeline

September 15, 2025
Want less conflict in your relationship? Try this simple perspective shift
Relationships and Sexual Health

“Love doesn’t thrive on ledgers”: Keeping score in relationships foreshadows decline, study finds

September 14, 2025
Study uncovers a gendered double standard for interracial relationships
Attachment Styles

Attachment insecurity shapes mentalization in interracial long-distance relationships

September 13, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New research finds the cumulative weight of social hardship across a lifespan shapes the aging brain

U.S. sees 5.7 million more childless women than expected, fueling a “demographic cliff”

AI hate speech detectors show major inconsistencies, new study reveals

New study sheds light on how sexual self-disclosure relates to relationship quality

Brain scan study connects parahippocampal cortex thinning with depression and neuroticism

People experiencing manic episodes have measurably higher skin temperatures

Higher cognitive ability and other psychological factors predict support for free speech

Autistic individuals and those with social anxiety differ in how they experience empathy, new study suggests

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy