Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Populism may act as a “thermometer” for democratic health

by Eric W. Dolan
August 8, 2025
in Political Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

A new study published in Political Psychology suggests that citizens’ populist attitudes are not fixed traits but instead shift over time, with changes often linked to how people view the performance of their country’s democracy. The findings imply that populist beliefs may act as a kind of “thermometer,” reflecting perceived democratic quality rather than remaining stable regardless of political conditions.

Marie-Isabel Theuwis, a postdoctoral researcher at the Université libre de Bruxelles, set out to better understand whether populist attitudes can actually change, and if so, what drives those changes.

Populism is a political approach that frames society as divided into two opposing groups: “the pure people” and “the corrupt elite.” It argues that politics should directly reflect the will of the people, often rejecting established representative institutions and compromises. Populist rhetoric can come from across the political spectrum, with leaders or movements portraying themselves as the true voice of ordinary citizens against an untrustworthy establishment.

Many political scientists view populism among citizens as a challenge for liberal democracies because it rejects representative decision-making in favor of direct control by “the people.” Some have proposed reforms to make political systems more inclusive and responsive in hopes of reducing populist discontent. But until now, it has been unclear whether such reforms could realistically influence citizens’ beliefs—or whether those beliefs are essentially locked in.

“Populism amongst citizens usually goes hand in hand with a sense of democratic dissatisfaction,” Theuwis explained. “I therefore study how changes in our democratic system affect our populist sentiments. A crucial piece in this research puzzle is to understand whether our populist attitudes are even changeable over time or if they represent a stable political disposition, and, if they do change, to explore why they would change.”

For her study, Theuwis analyzed two large panel datasets that tracked the same individuals over nearly four years: one from the Netherlands, which included 16 survey waves between 2020 and 2023, and one from Great Britain, which included six waves between 2016 and 2020. Both countries regularly measured populist attitudes using the same survey items, allowing for cross-national comparisons. The two political systems differ sharply—Britain’s is a majoritarian system dominated by a few parties, while the Netherlands has a highly proportional system with many parties—offering a chance to see if populist beliefs behave differently under these conditions.

The study measured populist attitudes with questions assessing agreement with statements such as “The politicians in Parliament need to follow the will of the people” and “What people call ‘compromise’ in politics is really just selling out on one’s principles.” Responses were averaged to produce a score from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree).

In addition to tracking these scores over time, Theuwis also measured variables that could influence them. These included satisfaction with democracy, satisfaction with the economy, and whether the respondent’s preferred party was in government—all indicators of perceived political performance. She also used indirect measures of “receptiveness to populist cues,” such as liking for a populist political leader and attachment to a populist party.

The statistical approach relied on latent growth curve models, which make it possible to assess how attitudes change within individuals over time and how those changes align with shifts in other variables. By comparing results to benchmark attitudes—political ideology, which is typically stable, and government satisfaction, which tends to fluctuate—Theuwis could determine where populist attitudes fall on the stability spectrum.

The results showed that populist attitudes are neither highly stable nor wildly volatile. In both Britain and the Netherlands, they changed more than political ideology but less than satisfaction with the government. The best-fitting statistical models indicated that changes were not gradual and linear but fluctuated over time, suggesting that shifts can occur in response to particular events or circumstances.

“Much like the temperature, our populist attitudes fluctuate over time,” Theuwis told PsyPost. “Changes in our satisfaction with democracy are associated with changes in our populist attitudes. This means that we should keep on trying to improve our democracies, since populist attitudes seem receptive to changes in the political system’s performance.”

When it came to what drives these shifts, patterns varied by country. In the Netherlands, changes in populist attitudes were consistently tied to changes in how people rated their democracy and, to a lesser extent, the economy. When satisfaction with democratic functioning or economic conditions improved, populist attitudes tended to decrease.

In Britain, democratic satisfaction also predicted changes in populist attitudes, but there was an added effect from supply-side influences. People who grew more attached to populist parties or expressed warmer feelings toward populist politicians were more likely to see their populist attitudes rise in subsequent surveys.

This difference may reflect the political contexts of each country. Britain’s majoritarian system and the high-profile Brexit campaign may have amplified the ability of populist leaders and parties to shape citizens’ views. In the Netherlands, with its multiparty system, shifts in political performance evaluations may play a larger role.

Overall, the study’s most consistent finding across both countries was that changes in democratic satisfaction were linked to changes in populist attitudes. This supports the idea that citizens’ populist beliefs partly reflect their evaluations of how well their democracy is functioning, rather than being fixed personality-driven worldviews.

Theuwis notes that the findings have implications for efforts to address populist discontent. If populist attitudes respond to perceived improvements in democratic quality, reforms that make political systems more inclusive, responsive, and representative could reduce the appeal of populist messages. At the same time, the study highlights the need to better understand the interplay between citizens’ beliefs and political events, as the relationship may work both ways—dissatisfaction can drive populism, but populist sentiment might also deepen democratic discontent.

The research does have limitations. It relied on proxy measures for exposure to populist cues, which may underestimate their influence, and it focused on just two countries. The time gaps between survey waves also mean it may have missed shorter-term shifts.

“I study the average change: not everybody’s populist attitudes behave the same. For some citizens, their populist attitudes remain stable, while for others, they change a lot over time.
I explored only some of many possible drivers of populist attitudes change. These other drivers are still left to explore.”

Future studies could track attitudes at more frequent intervals, test other potential drivers such as perceptions of corruption or social inequality, and explore whether populist attitudes change differently in countries where populist parties move in and out of government.

“In a study linked to this research (Theuwis & Kindt, 2024), with my colleague Rosa Kindt we assess whether processes that involve citizens more in democratic decision-making actually affect their populist attitudes,” Theuwis added. “We find that highly populist citizens become less populist after participation in such a participatory process. Then, in another study (Theuwis, 2025), I explore why such processes affect our populist attitudes, and what this means for how we should improve our democratic systems. I find that highly populist citizens pay special attention to whether authorities take citizens seriously. They want the political process to be inclusive and for citizens to have a visible impact on policy-making.”

The study, “Taking democracy’s temperature: The (in)stability and covariates of populist attitudes,” was published July 4, 2025.

RELATED

Professors who use safe space language seen as more caring—and more authoritarian
Authoritarianism

Professors who use safe space language seen as more caring—and more authoritarian

August 7, 2025

A new study finds that safe space statements can make students feel more comfortable and open in the classroom—but also make instructors seem more liberal and authoritarian. Trigger warnings, by contrast, had no meaningful impact on students’ perceptions.

Read moreDetails
Social class shapes perceptions of societal contribution
Conspiracy Theories

Worsening economic conditions fuel anti-immigrant conspiracy beliefs and support for violence

August 7, 2025

Belief in anti-immigrant conspiracies may be fueled by financial hardship and a sense of societal breakdown, according to new research. Across six studies, economic stress predicted support for discriminatory policies and even violent actions against non-European immigrants.

Read moreDetails
Common “cat poop” parasite hijacks brain chemistry through infected neuron vesicles
Political Psychology

Most Americans prefer a more diverse nation than the one they currently live in

August 6, 2025

Despite growing media focus on demographic anxiety and ethnonationalism, a new study finds most Americans envision a future United States that is more ethnically, racially, and religiously diverse. Very few support the idea of a homogenous national identity.

Read moreDetails
Study: Racist and sexist views were linked long before Obama
Political Psychology

Study: Racist and sexist views were linked long before Obama

August 4, 2025

A new study analyzing two decades of election data finds that racial resentment and sexist attitudes among white Americans are consistently linked. The findings suggest these views stem from a shared worldview, not just recent political events or figures.

Read moreDetails
Generative AI simplifies science communication, boosts public trust in scientists
Artificial Intelligence

Conservatives are more receptive to AI-generated recommendations than liberals, study finds

August 4, 2025

Contrary to popular belief, conservatives may be more receptive to AI in everyday life. A series of studies finds that conservatives are more likely than liberals to accept AI-generated recommendations.

Read moreDetails
Psilocybin helped aging mice not just live longer but also “look better” in groundbreaking new study
Political Psychology

Congressional speeches have shifted away from evidence-based rhetoric

July 31, 2025

A new study analyzing over eight million congressional speeches reveals a sharp decline in evidence-based language since the 1970s, raising concerns about rising political polarization, legislative gridlock, and growing income inequality in the United States.

Read moreDetails
Cross-party friendships are shockingly rare in the United States, study suggests
Political Psychology

Cross-party friendships are shockingly rare in the United States, study suggests

July 27, 2025

Most American friendships happen between people who share similar political beliefs, according to new research. But when political disagreement does exist between friends, it’s associated with less negative views of political opponents—even if the friendships themselves are a little less satisfying.

Read moreDetails
Study shows Congressional stock gains come at democracy’s expense
Political Psychology

Study shows Congressional stock gains come at democracy’s expense

July 25, 2025

New research shows that when Americans learn about members of Congress profiting from stock trades, they view lawmakers as more corrupt and less legitimate—and become less willing to follow the laws Congress passes.

Read moreDetails

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Only premium subscribers can comment — log in or join now.

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Childhood trauma tied to sexual narcissism and hypersexual behavior

Populism may act as a “thermometer” for democratic health

Melatonin supplements may boost DNA repair in night shift workers, new study finds

Bright children from low-income homes lose cognitive edge in early secondary school

New research supports the “rabbit hole” model of conspiratorial thinking

Moderate aerobic exercise enhances the brain’s “eighth sense”

Scientists have uncovered these weird facts about psychopathic individuals

Toddler irritability tied to gut microbiome diversity in the first weeks of life

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy