Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Dark Triad Psychopathy

Psychopathic traits linked to distinct cognitive disruptions in learning

by Eric W. Dolan
April 30, 2025
in Psychopathy
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook

A new study published in Translational Psychiatry has found that people with elevated psychopathic traits show distinct disruptions in how they learn from rewards and punishments. Rather than uniformly struggling to process feedback, different psychopathic traits were linked to specific patterns of impaired learning and biased expectations about change in the environment.

Psychopathic traits include characteristics like superficial charm, impulsivity, lack of empathy, and antisocial behavior. Although often associated with criminal behavior, these traits exist on a continuum and can be measured across the general population.

Previous research has suggested that people high in psychopathic traits often make risky or harmful decisions because they have difficulty learning from their mistakes. However, most studies have used simple tasks with only two options to choose from, which does not fully capture the complex decisions people face in everyday life. The researchers in the new study wanted to better understand how psychopathic traits are related to learning in more naturalistic, real-world situations where many options must be weighed.

“Psychopathy is often linked to reduced punishment sensitivity and poor behavioral adaptation. I’m interested in the cognitive and neural mechanisms behind this, particularly why individuals with psychopathic traits fail to adjust when associations between choices and outcomes change (for instance, when a previously rewarded choice starts resulting in punishments),” explained study author Dimana Atanassova, a postdoctoral researcher at the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour.

“One possibility is that they have impairments in volatility learning—the ability to detect and adapt to shifts in associations between choices and outcomes. While they can learn basic associations, they often struggle to update them when rewards or punishments reverse. In this study, I wanted to explore what happens in the brain when people have to learn new associations, and whether certain types of rewards and punishments might be more effective in this learning process.”

To investigate this, researchers recruited a community sample of 108 adults ranging in age from 19 to 54. Participants completed a foraging-style decision-making task designed to mimic real-world learning, where they had to repeatedly choose whether to stay with a known option or explore new ones. The outcomes were either monetary rewards and losses or personalized rewards and calibrated painful electric shocks, depending on the condition.

During the task, participants’ brain activity was recorded using electroencephalography (EEG), focusing on a brain signal known as feedback-related negativity, which is thought to reflect how the brain processes unexpected outcomes. Researchers also assessed psychopathic traits using a standard questionnaire measuring interpersonal, affective, lifestyle, and antisocial tendencies.

The study applied a computational model known as the Hierarchical Gaussian Filter to track how participants formed predictions about the environment and updated these predictions when receiving feedback. Specifically, the model allowed the researchers to distinguish between learning based on immediate outcomes and learning based on broader patterns of environmental change, sometimes called volatility tracking.

The researchers found that people higher in antisocial traits tended to believe the environment was more unstable than it really was. They expected that rules and patterns would change frequently, which can lead to poor learning because it makes feedback seem less meaningful. This finding helps explain why individuals with antisocial tendencies often show persistence in maladaptive behaviors, even when the environment signals that a change is necessary.

“I was surprised to find that individuals with higher antisocial traits—those more prone to risky or rule-breaking behavior even without a criminal record—tended to expect the world around them to change very rapidly,” Atanassova told PsyPost. “It was as if they perceived their environment as so unpredictable that forming stable associations between actions and outcomes became nearly impossible. This stood out even more when compared to others in our sample, who were able to adapt and learn from the task much more effectively.”

The researchers also found that higher interpersonal psychopathic traits—characterized by superficial charm and manipulativeness—were linked to weaker learning from personally meaningful rewards. These individuals were less responsive to outcomes that should have been motivating, which may reflect a diminished sensitivity to positive reinforcement.

In contrast, higher psychopathic affective traits—such as callousness and lack of empathy—were linked to reduced learning from painful punishments. Individuals with elevated affective traits showed muted brain responses when painful outcomes occurred, suggesting that they did not adjust their behavior effectively after experiencing punishment. However, they showed stronger learning from personalized rewards, suggesting that naturalistic, meaningful incentives could partly overcome some of the learning deficits seen in this group.

Interestingly, the study also found that a particular brain signal, the feedback-related negativity, tracked not immediate wins or losses, but rather participants’ evolving predictions about how the environment might be changing. This signal was sensitive to how surprising feedback was relative to expectations about change, rather than simply reflecting whether an outcome was good or bad. The strength of this brain signal also varied depending on participants’ psychopathic traits, highlighting how these traits shape not just behavior but also the brain’s basic learning processes.

The results offer a nuanced picture of how psychopathic traits impact learning. Rather than a general insensitivity to feedback, specific traits are linked to distinct patterns of learning impairments. Antisocial traits bias people to see the world as more volatile and unpredictable. Interpersonal traits blunt the motivational pull of rewards. Affective traits weaken the impact of punishments, particularly painful ones, while leaving learning from rewards relatively intact.

“Not all people with high psychopathic traits learn the same way,” Atanassova explained. “Some are more affected by punishments, while others are more influenced by rewards. For example, people with more manipulative psychopathic traits responded well to painful punishments but seemed rather unmotivated by rewards. On the other hand, those lacking empathy didn’t react much to punishment and displayed more reward-driven behavior.”

“These differences also show up in the brain, especially in how people detect and adapt to changes in consequences. These results challenge the perspective that all people with psychopathic traits are punishment-insensitive and highlight the importance of finding the right motivator; as such the findings might be important for future research focused on how to rehabilitate people with psychopathic traits.”

The researchers noted some limitations. Their sample consisted of healthy community adults rather than individuals diagnosed with clinical psychopathy or involved in the criminal justice system. Although psychopathic traits are distributed throughout the population, future research could examine whether similar patterns are observed in clinical or forensic groups.

“Still, studying these traits in everyday people helps us understand how they affect learning and behavior more broadly, especially since psychopathic traits in the general population are also associated with heightened risk of aggression and violence,” Atanassova said.

“This study demonstrated that people with psychopathic traits detect and adapt to change in the environment differently—for some, because punitive outcomes are not really salient, for some, because they’re insensitive to reward, and for others because they have a bias that makes them think the world changes far too rapidly to be predicted. As next steps, we’d like to understand where those impairments come from: is there a genetic/biological element to them, perhaps heightened by certain early life experiences?”

The study, “Exploring when to exploit: the cognitive underpinnings of foraging-type decisions in relation to psychopathy,” was authored by D. V. Atanassova, J. M. Oosterman, A. O. Diaconescu, C. Mathys, V. I. Madariaga, and I. A. Brazil.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Of all the dark personality traits, this one shows the strongest link to crime
Psychopathy

Violent offenders more likely to perceive ambiguous faces as angry, study shows

April 27, 2025

A new study in Psychological Medicine found no evidence that violent offenders have impaired fear processing, but showed they are more likely to perceive ambiguous faces as angry.

Read moreDetails
Some with psychopathic traits stay out of trouble — here’s what may explain the difference
Developmental Psychology

Some with psychopathic traits stay out of trouble — here’s what may explain the difference

April 20, 2025

A new study finds that early life experiences can influence whether people high in psychopathy engage in criminal behavior.

Read moreDetails
New study connects Mediterranean diet to positive brain chemistry
Mental Health

Youth with traumatic brain injuries show higher levels of psychopathic traits, new study finds

April 15, 2025

New research shows that head injuries in justice-involved youth are linked to more severe psychopathic traits, with substance use and cognitive problems playing a key role in the connection.

Read moreDetails
A core trait of psychopathy has a strange link to how the brain processes faces
Neuroimaging

A core trait of psychopathy has a strange link to how the brain processes faces

April 4, 2025

Neuroscientists have identified a link between psychopathic meanness and reduced brain activity during face perception.

Read moreDetails
The dark side of dominance: Victory can fuel sexual aggression in psychopathic men
Psychopathy

The dark side of dominance: Victory can fuel sexual aggression in psychopathic men

March 31, 2025

New research finds that some men become more sexually aggressive after winning a competition—especially those with cold, dominant personality traits.

Read moreDetails
People with psychopathic traits feel less pain—and assume others do too
Psychopathy

People with psychopathic traits feel less pain—and assume others do too

March 22, 2025

Reduced sensitivity to pain could be the key to understanding empathy deficits in individuals with psychopathic traits, a recent study suggests.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study shows how a dark trait predicts a disturbing kind of pleasure
Psychopathy

New psychology study shows how a dark trait predicts a disturbing kind of pleasure

March 9, 2025

A study found that psychopathy, particularly its coldheartedness component, strongly predicts enjoyment derived from harming others. This suggests emotional detachment plays a key role in everyday, non-sexual sadism.

Read moreDetails
Study examines link between psychopathic traits and emotional memory
Psychopathy

Study examines link between psychopathic traits and emotional memory

December 4, 2024

A study found no significant association between psychopathic traits and recall or recognition of emotional or neutral stimuli, suggesting that psychopathy does not impair general memory or preferential emotional memory processing in male inmates.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists challenge “dopamine detox” trend with evidence from avoidance learning

Non-right-handedness is more common across multiple mental health conditions

Withdrawal symptoms are common after stopping antidepressants

Anxious attachment linked to depression spillover in romantic relationships, study finds

Scientists find 16 new Alzheimer’s-linked genes using multi-ancestry genome data

Groundbreaking AI model uncovers hidden patterns of political bias in online news

Gut bacteria can influence how brain proteins are modified by carbohydrates

“Tiger mom” parenting boosts teens’ cognitive skills but undermines emotional development, study suggests

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy