Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Dark Triad Psychopathy

Secondary psychopathic traits linked to faster expansion of visual attention

by Eric W. Dolan
November 25, 2024
in Psychopathy
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

A recent study published in Personality and Individual Differences sheds light on the relationship between psychopathic traits and how individuals adjust their visual attention. Specifically, the research found that individuals with higher levels of antisocial tendencies, a key secondary psychopathic trait, tend to expand their attentional focus more quickly. This rapid shift suggests poorer attentional control or executive functioning.

Psychopathy is a personality construct characterized by distinct interpersonal, emotional, and behavioral traits. It is commonly divided into two subtypes: primary and secondary psychopathy, each associated with different patterns of thinking, feeling, and behaving.

Primary psychopathy involves traits such as emotional detachment, a lack of empathy, and egocentricity. Individuals high in primary psychopathic traits tend to be manipulative, callous, and focused on achieving their goals with little regard for others’ emotions. Secondary psychopathy, on the other hand, is characterized by impulsivity, poor self-regulation, and antisocial behaviors such as rule-breaking or aggression. These traits are often linked to difficulties in controlling attention and other executive functions.

Visual attentional breadth refers to how narrowly or broadly a person focuses their attention at a given moment. A narrow attentional breadth concentrates on small details within a scene, while a broad attentional breadth takes in a wider view, integrating information from a larger area. These attentional states are flexible and can shift depending on task demands or cognitive control. Since visual attention influences how people perceive and interact with their environment, examining its relationship with psychopathy could shed light on how individuals with psychopathic traits process the world around them.

“There are multiple dimensions of psychopathic personality traits, including egocentricity, callousness, impulsivity, and antisocial behavior. There is substantial diversity in the degree to which people exhibit these traits. We are interested in understanding the psychological mechanisms and processes that underpin these individual differences,” explained study authors Stephanie Goodhew and Mark Edwards, both associate professors at The Australian National University who co-direct the Visual Cognition Lab.

The researchers hypothesized that primary psychopathic traits might be associated with a narrowed attentional focus, aligning with the intense goal-directed behavior often seen in individuals with these traits. In contrast, secondary psychopathic traits might correlate with broader and less stable attentional control. To examine these relationships, the researchers conducted two experiments.

The first experiment aimed to determine whether psychopathic traits are linked to a preference for narrow or broad attentional breadth. The researchers recruited 118 participants from a community sample through an online platform known as Testable Minds. Participants completed a psychopathy assessment using the Expanded Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale. This scale measures three facets of psychopathy: egocentricity (a focus on self-interest), callousness (emotional detachment and lack of empathy), and antisociality (impulsivity and rule-breaking).

To measure attentional breadth, the participants completed a visual task using Navon stimuli—images where a larger “global” letter is composed of smaller “local” letters. In each trial, participants had to identify a target letter, which could appear at either the global or local level. Since participants were not directed to focus on a specific level, their responses were used to infer whether they naturally preferred a narrower attentional focus (favoring local details) or a broader one (favoring global shapes). Performance was measured by recording response times and accuracy for both levels.

The results showed no significant relationship between egocentricity or callousness (traits associated with primary psychopathy) and attentional breadth preference. However, those high in antisociality showed evidence of a weak preference for a broader attentional focus. These findings suggest that primary psychopathic traits are not strongly linked to attentional breadth, but secondary psychopathic traits might be.

In the second experiment, the researchers investigated how quickly individuals with different psychopathic traits could resize their attentional breadth. Resizing involves shifting attention between narrow and broad focus to meet changing demands, such as focusing on fine details in one moment and taking in a wider scene the next. This experiment used an adapted Navon task designed to measure how efficiently participants could contract or expand their attentional breadth.

A new group of 118 participants was recruited, and the same psychopathy assessment from Experiment 1 was used. Participants completed two blocks of trials with biased target probabilities. In one block, the majority of targets appeared at the global level (requiring broad focus), but a small proportion required participants to shift to a narrow focus to identify local targets. In another block, this setup was reversed, with the majority of targets at the local level and occasional global targets. The researchers recorded response times for the infrequent shifts, defining these as “contraction time” (broad to narrow) and “expansion time” (narrow to broad).

The results showed that individuals with higher levels of antisociality had faster expansion times, meaning they shifted from a narrow to broad attentional focus more quickly. This relationship was unique to antisociality and did not apply to egocentricity or callousness.

Interestingly, there was no significant link between antisociality and contraction time (broad to narrow). These findings suggest that antisociality, a secondary psychopathic trait, is associated with reduced attentional control, as quicker resizing may reflect less optimization of attentional focus for task demands.

“Our results indicate that people who demonstrate higher levels of impulsivity and antisocial behavior have difficulty regulating their focus of attention,” Goodhew and Edwards said.

Importantly, the finding that primary psychopathic traits, such as egocentricity and callousness, are not associated with a preference for a narrowed attentional breadth challenges the idea that attentional anomalies in individuals with these traits result from a naturally narrow focus of attention. Instead, this suggests that their difficulties with processing peripheral stimuli are more likely due to a bottleneck in cognitive processing capacity.

“Previous research has shown that people who demonstrate higher levels of egocentricity and callousness are more likely to process information that is relevant to their goals and not process goal-irrelevant information,” Goodhew and Edwards told PsyPost. “Initially, scientists thought that this might be because these people are particularly good at controlling their attention to focus on goals without succumbing to distraction.”

“But our research suggests that this is not the case, and instead our results are consistent with the alternative idea that higher egocentricity and callousness are associated with a reduced processing bandwidth. That is, these people are not processing distracting information because they do not have the capacity to do so, rather than because they are strategically filtering this out. In other words, all their capacity is consumed by processing the goal-relevant information.”

The study, like all research, has limitations. For example, the research relied on community samples rather than forensic populations, where extreme levels of psychopathy are more common. While this approach avoids potential biases linked to incarceration, it limits the range of psychopathic traits measured. Future studies could include participants from diverse populations to test whether these findings generalize to individuals with higher levels of primary psychopathic traits.

“As is common in psychological studies, these results describe patterns that emerge for the group as a whole,” the researchers noted. “This means that not every individual who scores high on the personality dimensions will exhibit these effects.”

Nevertheless, the findings contribute to a growing body of research exploring how cognitive mechanisms, such as attention, underpin psychopathic traits. Understanding the links between attention and psychopathy could inform interventions aimed at mitigating the negative consequences of these traits.

“Our lab’s research focus is on attentional control—how people can regulate their cognitive resources in goal-directed ways,” Goodhew and Edwards said. “We think that attentional control plays a pivotal role in people’s social and emotional dynamics. By elucidating this role, our aim is to find new ways to make people happier, healthier, and kinder.”

“We anticipate that improving our understanding of how attentional control is implicated in these psychopathic personality traits can inform interventions to reduce some of the negative consequences of these traits.”

The study, “On the relationship between psychopathic traits and visual attentional breadth,” was published online on October 2, 2024.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Psychopathy

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

July 12, 2025

Psychopathic traits alone don’t always lead to serious offending—but when combined with weak self-regulation, they may significantly raise the risk. A new study reveals how these factors interact to shape property crime patterns in adolescents already involved in the justice system.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns
Psychopathy

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

July 10, 2025

What if the roots of psychopathy could be traced in our blood? New research is looking beyond psychology and into our endocrine system. A new study suggests hormones like cortisol and testosterone may hold important clues to a person's manipulative and impulsive tendencies.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathic tendencies linked to reduced connectedness to nature and a preference for city-living
Psychopathy

Youth with psychopathic traits at increased risk of dying young, study finds

June 24, 2025

A long-term study of incarcerated teens has found that psychopathic personality traits are linked to a greater risk of dying young. The findings suggest these traits may be more predictive of premature mortality than conduct disorder or substance use.

Read moreDetails
Study helps untangle the complicated relationship between psychopathy and emotional awareness
Psychopathy

Psychopathic individuals recognize unfairness but are less likely to punish it

June 21, 2025

A new study shows that individuals with higher psychopathic traits are less likely to punish unfair behavior, especially when it costs them personally. The research suggests self-interest, not a lack of moral understanding, drives their reluctance to enforce social norms.

Read moreDetails
Maltreatment linked to altered brain structure and spontaneous thought patterns in children, study finds
Early Life Adversity and Childhood Maltreatment

Resilience may protect against psychopathic traits in people with childhood trauma

June 6, 2025

A new study suggests that people who are more resilient are less likely to develop psychopathic traits after experiencing childhood trauma. The findings highlight the protective role of resilience and its potential for reducing harmful personality patterns later in life.

Read moreDetails
Donald Trump’s presidency associated with significant changes in the topography of prejudice in the United States
Authoritarianism

Authoritarian beliefs predict whether voters see Trump or Clinton as psychopathic

June 4, 2025

Researchers found that voters’ authoritarian tendencies influenced how they judged the psychopathic traits of 2016 presidential candidates. Those high in authoritarianism were more likely to view Trump favorably and Clinton as psychologically disordered—and vice versa.

Read moreDetails
Study helps untangle the complicated relationship between psychopathy and emotional awareness
Psychopathy

Psychopathic traits linked to distinct cognitive disruptions in learning

April 30, 2025

A new study sheds light on the brain processes behind learning deficits in people with psychopathic traits. Researchers found that antisocial, interpersonal, and affective traits each relate differently to how individuals respond to rewards, punishments, and environmental changes

Read moreDetails
Of all the dark personality traits, this one shows the strongest link to crime
Psychopathy

Violent offenders more likely to perceive ambiguous faces as angry, study shows

April 27, 2025

A new study in Psychological Medicine found no evidence that violent offenders have impaired fear processing, but showed they are more likely to perceive ambiguous faces as angry.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Frequent egg consumption linked to lower risk of Alzheimer’s dementia, study finds

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

Low sexual activity, body shape, and mood may combine in ways that shorten lives, new study suggests

Highly irritable teens are more likely to bully others, but anxiety mitigates this tendency

Neuroscientists identify brain pathway that prioritizes safety over other needs

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

Neuroscientists shed new light on how heroin disrupts prefrontal brain function

New research identifies four distinct health pathways linked to Alzheimer’s disease

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy