Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Dark Triad Psychopathy

Violent offenders more likely to perceive ambiguous faces as angry, study shows

by Vladimir Hedrih
April 27, 2025
in Psychopathy
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A study conducted in Germany on male imprisoned violent offenders found no evidence that their fear processing is impaired. However, violent offenders, particularly those with pronounced aggression, were more likely to categorize certain highly ambiguous faces as angry compared to healthy controls. The research was published in Psychological Medicine.

Violent offenders are individuals who commit acts of physical aggression that cause or intend to cause harm to others, such as assault, homicide, or sexual violence. Psychologically, they tend to exhibit higher levels of impulsivity, making them more prone to act without considering consequences. Many struggle with emotional regulation, finding it difficult to manage anger, frustration, or fear. Some also display antisocial traits, including a lack of empathy, disregard for the rights of others, and manipulativeness.

Some violent offenders have histories of childhood trauma, abuse, or neglect, which can contribute to aggressive behavior later in life. Substance abuse is also common and can lower inhibitions or heighten aggression. While not all violent offenders have mental illnesses, certain disorders, such as borderline personality disorder or, in rare cases, schizophrenia, may increase the risk of violent behavior under specific conditions. Social and environmental factors—such as poverty, exposure to peer violence, or a lack of positive role models—often interact with psychological traits in the development of violent behavior.

Previous research has suggested that violent offenders may have impaired processing of fear, meaning it may be harder for them to experience fear themselves and to perceive it in others. To explore this possibility, study author Timo Stein and his colleagues conducted four experiments measuring how participants cognitively processed fearful and angry facial expressions, comparing their responses to neutral or other types of emotional expressions.

The study included 65 male imprisoned violent offenders and 60 age-matched control participants. The offenders, all convicted of violent crimes, were recruited from cooperating German correctional facilities. Among them, 21 individuals had sufficiently high scores on a psychopathy assessment to be classified as psychopathic.

Participants completed four cognitive tasks: two visual search tasks, one ambivalence task, and one morphing task. In the visual search tasks, participants were shown arrays of eight faces—seven identical distractors and one target face. In task one, the target differed by gender and emotional expression (happy, fearful, or neutral) or was tinted red. In task two, the design was the same except that red-tinted faces were excluded.

The ambivalence task used morphs blending angry, happy, and fearful expressions at different intensities. Participants had to identify the predominant emotion by pressing the corresponding button. In the morphing task, participants viewed a gradual transition from a neutral expression to a full emotional expression (happy, angry, fearful, or sad) and pressed a button as soon as they recognized any emotional change.

Analysis of participants’ responses across tasks found no evidence that violent offenders cognitively processed fear differently than healthy individuals. Similarly, individuals with high levels of aggression or psychopathy did not show a specific bias toward perceiving angry faces more readily.

However, in the ambivalence task, which required participants to explicitly identify the perceived emotion in highly ambiguous faces (those blending 50% happy and 50% angry expressions), violent offenders were significantly more likely than controls to categorize them as angry. This tendency was particularly pronounced in violent offenders who scored higher on measures of aggression.

However, in the ambivalence task, the task that required participants to explicitly identify the emotion they perceive, and when viewing highly ambiguous pictures (50% happy, 50% angry), violent offenders were more likely than controls to categorize them as angry. This was more pronounced in violent offenders with higher aggression levels.

“In conclusion, across four experiments we found no evidence for perceptual deficits for emotion (including fear) in psychopathy, but a cognitive bias for anger linked to aggression. These results challenge the view that psychopathy arises from altered emotion processing and support the idea that a hostile attribution bias may underlie aggressive behavior,” the  study authors concluded.

The study contributes to the scientific knowledge about visual perception and processing specificities of violent offenders. However, it should be noted that the study was conducted on a very small group of violent offenders who were all male. Results on female offenders might differ.

The paper, “Perception of emotional facial expressions in aggression and psychopathy,” was authored by Timo Stein, Nina Gehrer, Aiste Jusyte, Jonathan Scheeff, and Michael Schönenberg.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Study helps untangle the complicated relationship between psychopathy and emotional awareness
Psychopathy

Psychopathic traits linked to distinct cognitive disruptions in learning

April 30, 2025

A new study sheds light on the brain processes behind learning deficits in people with psychopathic traits. Researchers found that antisocial, interpersonal, and affective traits each relate differently to how individuals respond to rewards, punishments, and environmental changes

Read moreDetails
Some with psychopathic traits stay out of trouble — here’s what may explain the difference
Developmental Psychology

Some with psychopathic traits stay out of trouble — here’s what may explain the difference

April 20, 2025

A new study finds that early life experiences can influence whether people high in psychopathy engage in criminal behavior.

Read moreDetails
New study connects Mediterranean diet to positive brain chemistry
Mental Health

Youth with traumatic brain injuries show higher levels of psychopathic traits, new study finds

April 15, 2025

New research shows that head injuries in justice-involved youth are linked to more severe psychopathic traits, with substance use and cognitive problems playing a key role in the connection.

Read moreDetails
A core trait of psychopathy has a strange link to how the brain processes faces
Neuroimaging

A core trait of psychopathy has a strange link to how the brain processes faces

April 4, 2025

Neuroscientists have identified a link between psychopathic meanness and reduced brain activity during face perception.

Read moreDetails
The dark side of dominance: Victory can fuel sexual aggression in psychopathic men
Psychopathy

The dark side of dominance: Victory can fuel sexual aggression in psychopathic men

March 31, 2025

New research finds that some men become more sexually aggressive after winning a competition—especially those with cold, dominant personality traits.

Read moreDetails
People with psychopathic traits feel less pain—and assume others do too
Psychopathy

People with psychopathic traits feel less pain—and assume others do too

March 22, 2025

Reduced sensitivity to pain could be the key to understanding empathy deficits in individuals with psychopathic traits, a recent study suggests.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study shows how a dark trait predicts a disturbing kind of pleasure
Psychopathy

New psychology study shows how a dark trait predicts a disturbing kind of pleasure

March 9, 2025

A study found that psychopathy, particularly its coldheartedness component, strongly predicts enjoyment derived from harming others. This suggests emotional detachment plays a key role in everyday, non-sexual sadism.

Read moreDetails
Study examines link between psychopathic traits and emotional memory
Psychopathy

Study examines link between psychopathic traits and emotional memory

December 4, 2024

A study found no significant association between psychopathic traits and recall or recognition of emotional or neutral stimuli, suggesting that psychopathy does not impair general memory or preferential emotional memory processing in male inmates.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

What brain scans reveal about the neural correlates of pornography consumption

AI chatbots often misrepresent scientific studies — and newer models may be worse

Is gender-affirming care helping or harming mental health?

Study finds “zombie” neurons in the peripheral nervous system contribute to chronic pain

Therapeutic video game shows promise for post-COVID cognitive recovery

Passive scrolling linked to increased anxiety in teens, study finds

Your bodily awareness guides your morality, new neuroscience study suggests

Where you flirt matters: New research shows setting shapes romantic success

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy