Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

The startling power of political identity in shaping Americans’ social preferences

by Eric W. Dolan
May 2, 2025
in Political Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study published in Political Psychology suggests that in the United States, political identity is the strongest driver of how people view others in everyday social situations—outweighing other key social identities like race, religion, class, or education. Even in community settings unrelated to politics, partisanship appears to be the most powerful predictor of interpersonal preferences, revealing the extent to which polarization has moved beyond the political sphere and into social life.

The study examined how political identity compares to other social traits in predicting affective polarization, a term used to describe the growing dislike between members of different political parties. The researchers sought to understand whether this polarization is driven mainly by political affiliation or by other traits commonly associated with partisanship, such as ideology, race, and religion. They also wanted to assess whether individuals with “cross-cutting” identities—those who do not align neatly with party stereotypes—are less likely to display polarized attitudes.

“It’s well known that Americans are politically polarized,” said study author Julie Norman, an associate professor at University College London. “It’s also clear that Americans are increasingly living in different social bubbles that reflect not only political differences, but also other types of divides, including preferences related to class, race, religion, education attainment, and geography. We wanted to better understand what was most driving this division in American social life. Is it pure politics, or is it one (or more) of these other factors?”

To address these questions, the researchers conducted a large-scale conjoint survey experiment using a nationally representative sample of 1,330 participants in the United States. Respondents were repeatedly shown side-by-side profiles of two hypothetical people with randomly assigned traits, including political party, ideology, race, gender, religion, education level, income, and geographic region. After viewing each pair, participants were asked to rate how favorably they viewed each individual and to make choices about who they would prefer as a friend, neighbor, or family member by marriage. These social distance questions allowed the researchers to examine both abstract attitudes and more concrete social preferences.

The results showed that political party identity had the strongest and most consistent effect on participants’ responses. People expressed more favorable views of profiles that matched their own party affiliation and showed significantly less favorable attitudes toward those affiliated with the opposing party. This effect was especially strong in general favorability ratings but still present in social distance decisions, such as choosing a preferred neighbor or in-law.

In fact, political identity was a more powerful predictor of social preferences than race, religion, or gender. While religious identity did show some effects—particularly negative biases toward Muslims and atheists—the impact of party identification was even stronger. For example, Democrats consistently rated Democratic profiles more positively than independents or Republicans and showed pronounced dislike for Republican profiles. Republicans similarly favored members of their own party, although the degree of out-group dislike was less intense than among Democrats.

“Overall, our findings indicate that political partisanship is the primary driver of social divides in America today, outweighing race and ethnicity, class, level of education, geographic locale, and other demographic traits and identities that are so often subject to social biases,” Norman told PsyPost. “Many of these traits and identities are indeed intertwined with partisan identity, yet even when we untangle those overlapping identities, partisanship remains the strongest and most significant indicator of social preferences.”

One of the study’s more nuanced findings is that out-group animosity tended to be stronger than in-group favoritism when participants rated profiles in terms of favorability. In other words, people disliked the opposing party more than they liked their own. However, when it came to the interpersonal social distance questions—such as selecting a friend or neighbor—the pattern was more mixed. A deeper analysis using Bayesian modeling techniques found that in-group preference was sometimes more significant than out-group animosity in those behavioral measures, suggesting that while political bias is present in social decision-making, it may not always translate into outright hostility.

The researchers also explored whether individuals or profiles with cross-cutting identities—those who defy partisan stereotypes would generate less polarized reactions. Contrary to expectations, they found no evidence that these identities reduced polarization. Respondents were not more accepting of people with diverse or atypical combinations of traits, nor were individuals with cross-cutting identities themselves less polarized in their responses.

“We were especially interested in the impact of ‘cross-cutting identities,’ or profiles that do not fully conform to partisan stereotypes, such as Black Republicans, or evangelical Christian Democrats. Contrary to our assumptions, cross-cutting identities do not appear to dampen social polarization,” Norman explained. “People’s basic preference for someone with the same political affiliation as them applied even to seemingly ‘atypical’ Democrats or Republicans. And people who themselves had cross-cutting identities were still more likely to prefer their political group over the other.”

Another key finding was that while both ideology and partisanship influenced social preferences, political identity typically had a stronger effect than ideological labels such as “liberal” or “conservative.” This was especially true among Democrats and liberals, who showed more consistent alignment with their party. Among Republicans, conservative identity played a somewhat stronger role than party affiliation, suggesting a slightly different dynamic.

The study also uncovered differences in how each party responds to race and religion. Democrats rated Black profiles more favorably than other racial groups, while Republicans did not display a statistically significant preference or aversion based on race. For religion, Republicans rated Jewish, Muslim, and atheist profiles significantly lower than comparable Christian profiles. Democrats also rated atheists somewhat less favorably, but were more neutral toward Muslim and Jewish individuals.

“Aside from partisanship, religion emerged as the other key trait that guided people’s choices, but religion’s influence also depended on the respondent’s political affiliation,” Norman said. “Republicans viewed Jewish, Muslim, and atheist profiles more negatively than otherwise identical Christian profiles, and Democrats also viewed atheists more negatively than Christian profiles.”

“Regarding how partisan respondents view racial identity, Democrats viewed Black profiles more favorably than Republicans did. But importantly, this was because Democrats were uniquely positively inclined to Black profiles, not that Republicans viewed Black profiles especially negatively. Interestingly, White participants did not view Black profiles any differently than non-White participants did. So, the racial prejudices that some might expect in these situations were superseded by people’s political allegiances.”

Taken together, these findings show how strongly political identity now influences Americans’ everyday social preferences, often eclipsing other traditional social divisions. The study provides evidence that polarization in the United States is not just about policy or ideology—it’s about who people feel comfortable interacting with in their communities.

At the same time, the findings also suggest that affective polarization may be more limited in certain real-world contexts. While abstract ratings of favorability showed strong partisan bias, behavioral measures of social distance produced more ambiguous results. People may still be willing to befriend or live next to someone from the opposing party, even if they express negative attitudes in more general terms.

The study does have limitations. It relied on hypothetical profiles and survey responses, which may not fully capture how people behave in real life. Social desirability bias—participants modifying their responses to appear more tolerant—was minimized through the survey design, but it cannot be entirely ruled out. Additionally, while the researchers expected cross-cutting identities to play a moderating role, they found no such effect, raising questions about how to measure the influence of complex, overlapping identities in studies of polarization.

Future research may focus more directly on real-world behavior and explore how people navigate political differences in specific social settings. It may also examine how individuals define their identities and which ones are most meaningful to them in different contexts. More work is also needed to explore the role of elite messaging, media framing, and social networks in reinforcing or mitigating partisan divisions.

“While we focus on the identities motivating polarization, recent scholarship has shown that polarization between parties is not inevitable, but rather reflects an interplay between supply and demand factors that interact and reinforce each other,” Norman explained. “That is, it is not just our identities that drive polarization, but rather external cues from politicians, political parties, interest groups, media, social media, etc that activate those identities. In future studies, we’d be interested in building on emerging scholarship to investigate what kinds of messaging or primes helps counter polarization or mitigate social divides.”

The study, “Why can’t we be friends? Untangling conjoined polarization in America,” was authored by Julie M. Norman and Beniamino Green.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes
Narcissism

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

June 13, 2025

New research published in Behavioral Sciences shows that certain narcissistic traits are linked to anti-immigrant attitudes through competitive worldviews and ideological beliefs, highlighting the role of personality in shaping how people view immigrants and social hierarchies.

Read moreDetails
Uncertainty about immigration intensifies nationalist sentiments
Political Psychology

From well-being to white replacement: What psychology research says about immigration

June 11, 2025

Immigration sparks fierce debates—but what does the research say? These seven studies uncover surprising findings about mental health, crime, social media, nationalism, and political violence.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Exposure to heavy metals is associated with higher likelihood of ADHD diagnosis

Eye-tracking study shows people fixate longer on female aggressors than male ones

Romantic breakups follow a two-stage decline that begins years before the split, study finds

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

A common parasite not only invades the brain — it can also decapitate human sperm

Almost all unmarried pregant women say that the fetus resembles the father, study finds

New neuroscience research reveals brain antioxidant deficit in depression

Scientists uncover kidney-to-brain route for Parkinson’s-related protein spread

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy