Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Moral Psychology

Neurolinguistic priming reveals contrasting gender biases in Republicans and Democrats

by Eric W. Dolan
December 6, 2024
in Moral Psychology, Political Psychology, Sexism
(Photo credit: DALL·E)

(Photo credit: DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A recent study provides evidence that political ideology shapes how people evaluate moral violations based on gender, with Republicans and Democrats exhibiting contrasting biases. Republicans judged authority violations by women and girls more harshly, while Democrats tended to judge such violations by men and boys as worse. These findings, published in The Journal of Social Psychology, shed light on how implicit biases can differ significantly across political lines.

The study aimed to explore how implicit gender biases influence moral judgments differently among Republicans and Democrats, given their distinct moral priorities and social values. By manipulating the timing of gender information in moral violation scenarios, the researchers sought to uncover how political ideology and framing effects shape evaluations of authority violations.

“When I was teaching biology in rural Kansas, I got really interested in how people maintain beliefs in the face of counterevidence. Since then, I have been looking specifically at how in-group membership influences cognition and beliefs,” said study author Brandon L. Bretl, an assistant professor at the University of Texas at Tyler.

The researchers employed an experimental survey design to investigate how political ideology influences gender biases in moral judgments. They recruited 826 participants from a United States census-matched sample, ensuring diverse representation in terms of age, gender, and political affiliation. The participants self-identified as either Democrats or Republicans, excluding independents.

The survey included a series of short moral violation scenarios, known as vignettes, designed to test reactions in two domains: authority violations, such as a student interrupting a teacher, and justice violations, such as cheating during a test.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions that manipulated the gender of the vignette’s protagonist and the placement of gender-related information within the sentences. In some conditions, gender was introduced early in the vignette using a noun, while in others, it was introduced later through a pronoun. This manipulation aimed to test the influence of timing on gender stereotype activation.

“Our brains use different neural circuits for different tasks,” Bretl told PsyPost. “The processing done by those circuits happens very quickly—often in less than a tenth of a second. Even so, some processes don’t happen quickly enough in certain contexts. For example, in this study, I asked people to judge written scenarios of men and women committing moral violations. I then changed the gender of the person committing the violation between two experimental conditions.

“I also varied the placement of gender information in two additional conditions, presenting it either at the beginning of the sentence with a noun (‘You see a woman…’) or later in the sentence using a pronoun (‘You see a student…her…’). The idea was that moral judgments occur rapidly, so in cases where we expect a gender bias—for example, a woman disrespecting authority—we could mitigate that bias by presenting gender information later in the sentence.”

After reading each vignette, participants rated the severity of the moral violation on a five-point scale, ranging from “not bad” to “extremely bad.” To ensure results were not influenced by unrelated factors, the researchers controlled for variables such as participant sex, religiosity, and the strength of political affiliation.

The findings revealed distinct patterns of gender bias influenced by political ideology. Republicans rated authority violations by women and girls as more severe than those committed by men and boys when gender information appeared early in the sentence. This pattern suggests that traditional gender norms emphasizing female deference to authority may be particularly salient among Republican participants. Interestingly, this bias disappeared when gender information was introduced later in the sentence, implying that subtle shifts in how information is framed can reduce implicit biases.

In contrast, Democrats showed the opposite bias in their evaluations of authority violations. They rated male protagonists as more culpable than female protagonists, particularly when gender was introduced early in the vignette. This pattern may reflect a sensitivity to social equity issues and an overcorrection for perceived biases against women, resulting in harsher judgments of male authority violators. As with Republicans, the timing of gender information affected these judgments, with late gender cues reducing the observed bias.

“The bias against boys and men committing authority violations among Democrats surprised me,” Bretl said. “One explanation is an ‘overcorrection bias,’ where Democrats, being aware of biases against women, have worked to overcome those biases but end up overshooting the mark. More research is needed to confirm this.”

In scenarios involving justice violations, such as cheating or dishonesty, neither Republicans nor Democrats exhibited significant gender bias. This finding underscores the domain-specific nature of implicit biases, as justice violations—focused on fairness and harm—may not activate the same gendered stereotypes as authority violations. The results highlight how moral judgments are shaped not only by political ideology but also by the context in which violations occur and the way information is presented.

“The key takeaways from this study are: 1) gender biases function in highly nuanced ways, 2) Republicans are biased against girls and women committing authority violations, while Democrats are biased against boys and men committing authority violations, and 3) the gender of the protagonist does not influence ratings of justice violations,” Bretl explained. “This is relevant in contexts like status offenses—crimes punishable because of the offender’s age, such as truancy or underage drinking—because these authority-based offenses are the only area of the criminal justice system where girls outnumber boys.”

The study introduces a novel method for examining implicit biases by manipulating the presentation of gender information in moral scenarios. However, the study has limitations. The vignettes primarily featured youth protagonists, which may not fully capture biases related to adult authority figures. Additionally, the findings are specific to the polarized political and cultural context of the United States in 2023.

“This study focused on U.S. participants during a time of heightened political polarization,” Bretl noted. “The findings might not generalize to other cultural or political contexts. Also, while we controlled for factors like age and religiosity, biases related to age or other demographic traits might still play a role.”

To build on these findings, future studies could examine how other social factors interact with gender and political ideology in shaping moral judgments. Researchers could also explore interventions to mitigate implicit biases, such as framing information in ways that neutralize stereotype activation.

“The long-term goals include developing a better understanding of how stereotypes and biases function in the brain and in context,” Bretl said. “Such knowledge can be used to improve outcomes in areas where biases can have undesirable effects, such as criminal sentencing and beliefs about science.”

“The study is unique in its blending of neurolinguistics and social psychology. The experimental design is relatively complex, but it needed to be to tease out the nuanced differences between Republicans’ and Democrats’ gender biases when making moral judgments.”

The paper, “Neurolinguistic Priming and Gender Stereotype Effects in the Ratings of Justice vs. Authority Moral Violations: Republicans and Democrats,” was authored by Brandon L. Bretl and Christopher L. Thomas.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails
Gynandromorph research offers insight into the complexities of male sexual attraction
Sexism

Eye-tracking study shows people fixate longer on female aggressors than male ones

June 23, 2025

People pay more visual attention to female aggressors than male ones, but do not consistently judge their actions as more intentional or blameworthy, suggesting that female aggression is seen as unexpected rather than more morally significant.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New study reveals how MDMA rewires serotonin and oxytocin systems in the brain

Ghosting and ‘breadcrumbing’: the psychological impact of our bad behaviour on dating apps

Older adults who feel criticized by loved ones are more likely to develop depression

New study exposes gap between ADHD drug use and safety research in children

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

Love addiction linked to memory and attention problems

Positive early experiences may buffer suicidal thoughts in those with trauma symptoms, new study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy